American Psychological Association Logo

Racism, bias, and discrimination

collage of Black man in wheelchair, Asian American mother and daughter, and Black woman holding a rainbow flag

Racism is a form of prejudice that generally includes negative emotional reactions to members of a group, acceptance of negative stereotypes, and racial discrimination against individuals; in some cases it can lead to violence.

Discrimination refers to the differential treatment of different age, gender, racial, ethnic, religious, national, ability identity, sexual orientation, socioeconomic, and other groups at the individual level and the institutional/structural level. Discrimination is usually the behavioral manifestation of prejudice and involves negative, hostile, and injurious treatment of members of rejected groups.

Adapted from the APA Dictionary of Psychology

Resources from APA

collage of racially diverse people

Racial Equity Action Plan Progress and Impact Report

Update on APA’s efforts toward dismantling systemic racism in psychology and society

Dr. Alfiee Breland-Noble

Empowering youth of color

The psychologist is one of 12 global leaders who received a $20 million grant from Melinda French Gates.

Therapist works with man in wheelchair who has Down syndrome.

Implicit theories concerning the intelligence of individuals with Down syndrome

Think professionals who work with people with disabilities are immune to bias? Think again

professional talking with a client

Combating stigma against patients

How clinicians can overcome bias and provide better treatment

More resources about racism

What APA is doing

Religion, Race & Ethnicity

Race, ethnicity, and religion

APA Services advocates for the equal treatment of people of all races, religions, and ethnicities, as well as funding for federal programs that address health disparities in these groups.

Equity, diversity, and inclusion

Inclusive language guidelines

APA’s commitment to addressing systemic racism

APA’s action plan for addressing inequality

APA’s apology to people of color in the U.S.

Confronting past wrongs and building an equitable future

Stigma of Disease and Disability

The Psychology of Hate

Microaggressions and Traumatic Stress

Addressing Cultural Complexities in Counseling and Clinical Practice

Trauma and Racial Minority Immigrants

Magination Press children’s books

Bernice Sandler and the Fight for Title IX

Cover of Lulu the One and Only (medium)

Lulu the One and Only

Cover of There's a Cat in Our Class! (medium)

There's a Cat in Our Class!

Algo Le Pasó a Mi Papá

Something Happened to My Dad

Something Happened to My Dad

Journal special issues

Training and Educating Antiracist Psychologists

Asian America and the COVID-19 Pandemic

Dismantling Racism in the Field of Psychology and Beyond

Understanding, Unpacking and Eliminating Health Disparities

Recentering AAPI Narratives as Social Justice Praxis

Ethnic psychological associations

  • American Arab, Middle Eastern, and North African Psychological Association
  • The Association of Black Psychologists
  • Asian American Psychological Association
  • National Latinx Psychological Association
  • Society of Indian Psychologists

Related resources

  • Protecting and Defending our People: Nakni tushka anowa (The Warrior’s Path) Final Report (PDF, 8.64MB) APA Division 45 Warrior’s Path Presidential Task Force (2020)
  • Society for Community Research and Action (APA Division 27) Antiracism / Antioppression email series
  • Society of Counseling Psychology (APA Division 17) Social justice resources
  • Talking About Race | National Museum of African American History and Culture Tools and guidance for discussions of race
  • InnoPsych therapists of color search

Featured Topics

Featured series.

A series of random questions answered by Harvard experts.

Explore the Gazette

Read the latest.

essay on racial bias

Toll of QAnon on families of followers

Group pf boys standing.

The urgent message coming from boys

essay on racial bias

Your kid can’t name three branches of government? He’s not alone. 

Mahzarin Banaji

Mahzarin Banaji opened the symposium on Tuesday by recounting the “implicit association” experiments she had done at Yale and at Harvard. The final talk is today at 9 a.m.

Kris Snibbe/Harvard Staff Photographer

Turning a light on our implicit biases

Brett Milano

Harvard Correspondent

Social psychologist details research at University-wide faculty seminar

Few people would readily admit that they’re biased when it comes to race, gender, age, class, or nationality. But virtually all of us have such biases, even if we aren’t consciously aware of them, according to Mahzarin Banaji, Cabot Professor of Social Ethics in the Department of Psychology, who studies implicit biases. The trick is figuring out what they are so that we can interfere with their influence on our behavior.

Banaji was the featured speaker at an online seminar Tuesday, “Blindspot: Hidden Biases of Good People,” which was also the title of Banaji’s 2013 book, written with Anthony Greenwald. The presentation was part of Harvard’s first-ever University-wide faculty seminar.

“Precipitated in part by the national reckoning over race, in the wake of George Floyd, Breonna Taylor and others, the phrase ‘implicit bias’ has almost become a household word,” said moderator Judith Singer, Harvard’s senior vice provost for faculty development and diversity. Owing to the high interest on campus, Banaji was slated to present her talk on three different occasions, with the final one at 9 a.m. Thursday.

Banaji opened on Tuesday by recounting the “implicit association” experiments she had done at Yale and at Harvard. The assumptions underlying the research on implicit bias derive from well-established theories of learning and memory and the empirical results are derived from tasks that have their roots in experimental psychology and neuroscience. Banaji’s first experiments found, not surprisingly, that New Englanders associated good things with the Red Sox and bad things with the Yankees.

She then went further by replacing the sports teams with gay and straight, thin and fat, and Black and white. The responses were sometimes surprising: Shown a group of white and Asian faces, a test group at Yale associated the former more with American symbols though all the images were of U.S. citizens. In a further study, the faces of American-born celebrities of Asian descent were associated as less American than those of white celebrities who were in fact European. “This shows how discrepant our implicit bias is from even factual information,” she said.

How can an institution that is almost 400 years old not reveal a history of biases, Banaji said, citing President Charles Eliot’s words on Dexter Gate: “Depart to serve better thy country and thy kind” and asking the audience to think about what he may have meant by the last two words.

She cited Harvard’s current admission strategy of seeking geographic and economic diversity as examples of clear progress — if, as she said, “we are truly interested in bringing the best to Harvard.” She added, “We take these actions consciously, not because they are easy but  because they are in our interest and in the interest of society.”

Moving beyond racial issues, Banaji suggested that we sometimes see only what we believe we should see. To illustrate she showed a video clip of a basketball game and asked the audience to count the number of passes between players. Then the psychologist pointed out that something else had occurred in the video — a woman with an umbrella had walked through — but most watchers failed to register it. “You watch the video with a set of expectations, one of which is that a woman with an umbrella will not walk through a basketball game. When the data contradicts an expectation, the data doesn’t always win.”

Expectations, based on experience, may create associations such as “Valley Girl Uptalk” is the equivalent of “not too bright.” But when a quirky way of speaking spreads to a large number of young people from certain generations,  it stops being a useful guide. And yet, Banaji said, she has been caught in her dismissal of a great idea presented in uptalk.  Banaji stressed that the appropriate course of action is not to ask the person to change the way she speaks but rather for her and other decision makers to know that using language and accents to judge ideas is something people at their own peril.

Banaji closed the talk with a personal story that showed how subtler biases work: She’d once turned down an interview because she had issues with the magazine for which the journalist worked.

The writer accepted this and mentioned she’d been at Yale when Banaji taught there. The professor then surprised herself by agreeing to the interview based on this fragment of shared history that ought not to have influenced her. She urged her colleagues to think about positive actions, such as helping that perpetuate the status quo.

“You and I don’t discriminate the way our ancestors did,” she said. “We don’t go around hurting people who are not members of our own group. We do it in a very civilized way: We discriminate by who we help. The question we should be asking is, ‘Where is my help landing? Is it landing on the most deserved, or just on the one I shared a ZIP code with for four years?’”

To subscribe to short educational modules that help to combat implicit biases, visit outsmartinghumanminds.org .

Share this article

You might like.

essay on racial bias

New book by Nieman Fellow explores pain, frustration in efforts to help loved ones break free of hold of conspiracy theorists

Group pf boys standing.

When we don’t listen, we all suffer, says psychologist whose new book is ‘Rebels with a Cause’

essay on racial bias

Efforts launched to turn around plummeting student scores in U.S. history, civics, amid declining citizen engagement across nation

You want to be boss. You probably won’t be good at it.

Study pinpoints two measures that predict effective managers

Good genes are nice, but joy is better

Harvard study, almost 80 years old, has proved that embracing community helps us live longer, and be happier

Home — Essay Samples — Social Issues — Discrimination and Prejudice — Racial Discrimination

one px

Essays on Racial Discrimination

Themes in lorraine hansberry's "a raisin in the sun", racial discrimination and sexism in william shakespeare's plays, made-to-order essay as fast as you need it.

Each essay is customized to cater to your unique preferences

+ experts online

The Issue of Racial Discrimination in Beyoncé's Song "Formation"

Racial discrimination and microaggressions and its consequences, effects of racial discrimination on health, employment, and housing in the us, racism in society, its effects and ways to overcome, let us write you an essay from scratch.

  • 450+ experts on 30 subjects ready to help
  • Custom essay delivered in as few as 3 hours

Critical Analysis of Kaepernicks Act of Kneeling

Race discrimination and its existence nowadays, racism as a result of discrimination towards the minorities in a community, inequality in the marrow of tradition, get a personalized essay in under 3 hours.

Expert-written essays crafted with your exact needs in mind

Causes and Effects of Racism in Born a Crime

The hidden impact of internalized racism on psychology and society, the case of trayvon martin and stand-your-ground law in the us, the importance of analyzing racist depictions in films, black oppression in america: jim crow laws to today’s society , the flowers by alice walker: the impact of racism on children, trayvon martin case: self-defense vs deadly force, racial profiling: it’s not in the past, the difference between the lives of black and whites during slavery, the issue of racial prejudice in american literature, racial & gender inequalities in the workplace, workplace discrimination against african americans in the us, black boy by richard wright: being a minority in the deep south, african americans who started the battle for civil rights, gregory mantsio's 'class in america': the issue of prejudice in society, prejudice, racism and american dream in gregory mantsios' 'class in america', the overrepresentation of certain groups of society in the criminal justice system, conflicting views on the idea of affirmative action, racial stereotyping in the movie straight outta compton, rise of anti-muslim sentiment: islamophobia, relevant topics.

  • Racial Profiling
  • Discrimination
  • Gender Discrimination
  • Hate Speech
  • Fat Shaming
  • Multiculturalism
  • Transphobia
  • Gun Control
  • Martin Luther King

By clicking “Check Writers’ Offers”, you agree to our terms of service and privacy policy . We’ll occasionally send you promo and account related email

No need to pay just yet!

Bibliography

We use cookies to personalyze your web-site experience. By continuing we’ll assume you board with our cookie policy .

  • Instructions Followed To The Letter
  • Deadlines Met At Every Stage
  • Unique And Plagiarism Free

essay on racial bias

Stanford University

Along with Stanford news and stories, show me:

  • Student information
  • Faculty/Staff information

We want to provide announcements, events, leadership messages and resources that are relevant to you. Your selection is stored in a browser cookie which you can remove at any time using “Clear all personalization” below.

A pandemic that disproportionately affected communities of color, roadblocks that obstructed efforts to expand the franchise and protect voting discrimination, a growing movement to push anti-racist curricula out of schools – events over the past year have only underscored how prevalent systemic racism and bias is in America today.

What can be done to dismantle centuries of discrimination in the U.S.? How can a more equitable society be achieved? What makes racism such a complicated problem to solve? Black History Month is a time marked for honoring and reflecting on the experience of Black Americans, and it is also an opportunity to reexamine our nation’s deeply embedded racial problems and the possible solutions that could help build a more equitable society.

Stanford scholars are tackling these issues head-on in their research from the perspectives of history, education, law and other disciplines. For example, historian Clayborne Carson is working to preserve and promote the legacy of Martin Luther King Jr. and religious studies scholar Lerone A. Martin has joined Stanford to continue expanding access and opportunities to learn from King’s teachings; sociologist Matthew Clair is examining how the criminal justice system can end a vicious cycle involving the disparate treatment of Black men; and education scholar Subini Ancy Annamma is studying ways to make education more equitable for historically marginalized students.

Learn more about these efforts and other projects examining racism and discrimination in areas like health and medicine, technology and the workplace below.

Update: Jan. 27, 2023: This story was originally published on Feb. 16, 2021, and has been updated on a number of occasions to include new content.

Understanding the impact of racism; advancing justice

One of the hardest elements of advancing racial justice is helping everyone understand the ways in which they are involved in a system or structure that perpetuates racism, according to Stanford legal scholar Ralph Richard Banks.

“The starting point for the center is the recognition that racial inequality and division have long been the fault line of American society. Thus, addressing racial inequity is essential to sustaining our nation, and furthering its democratic aspirations,” said Banks , the Jackson Eli Reynolds Professor of Law at Stanford Law School and co-founder of the Stanford Center for Racial Justice .

This sentiment was echoed by Stanford researcher Rebecca Hetey . One of the obstacles in solving inequality is people’s attitudes towards it, Hetey said. “One of the barriers of reducing inequality is how some people justify and rationalize it.”

How people talk about race and stereotypes matters. Here is some of that scholarship.

For Black Americans, COVID-19 is quickly reversing crucial economic gains

Research co-authored by SIEPR’s Peter Klenow and Chad Jones measures the welfare gap between Black and white Americans and provides a way to analyze policies to narrow the divide.

How an ‘impact mindset’ unites activists of different races

A new study finds that people’s involvement with Black Lives Matter stems from an impulse that goes beyond identity.

For democracy to work, racial inequalities must be addressed

The Stanford Center for Racial Justice is taking a hard look at the policies perpetuating systemic racism in America today and asking how we can imagine a more equitable society.

The psychological toll of George Floyd’s murder

As the nation mourned the death of George Floyd, more Black Americans than white Americans felt angry or sad – a finding that reveals the racial disparities of grief.

Seven factors contributing to American racism

Of the seven factors the researchers identified, perhaps the most insidious is passivism or passive racism, which includes an apathy toward systems of racial advantage or denial that those systems even exist.

Scholars reflect on Black history

Humanities and social sciences scholars reflect on “Black history as American history” and its impact on their personal and professional lives.

The history of Black History Month

It's February, so many teachers and schools are taking time to celebrate Black History Month. According to Stanford historian Michael Hines, there are still misunderstandings and misconceptions about the past, present, and future of the celebration.

Numbers about inequality don’t speak for themselves

In a new research paper, Stanford scholars Rebecca Hetey and Jennifer Eberhardt propose new ways to talk about racial disparities that exist across society, from education to health care and criminal justice systems.

Changing how people perceive problems

Drawing on an extensive body of research, Stanford psychologist Gregory Walton lays out a roadmap to positively influence the way people think about themselves and the world around them. These changes could improve society, too.

Welfare opposition linked to threats of racial standing

Research co-authored by sociologist Robb Willer finds that when white Americans perceive threats to their status as the dominant demographic group, their resentment of minorities increases. This resentment leads to opposing welfare programs they believe will mainly benefit minority groups.

Conversations about race between Black and white friends can feel risky, but are valuable

New research about how friends approach talking about their race-related experiences with each other reveals concerns but also the potential that these conversations have to strengthen relationships and further intergroup learning.

Defusing racial bias

Research shows why understanding the source of discrimination matters.

Many white parents aren’t having ‘the talk’ about race with their kids

After George Floyd’s murder, Black parents talked about race and racism with their kids more. White parents did not and were more likely to give their kids colorblind messages.

Stereotyping makes people more likely to act badly

Even slight cues, like reading a negative stereotype about your race or gender, can have an impact.

Why white people downplay their individual racial privileges

Research shows that white Americans, when faced with evidence of racial privilege, deny that they have benefited personally.

Clayborne Carson: Looking back at a legacy

Stanford historian Clayborne Carson reflects on a career dedicated to studying and preserving the legacy of civil rights leader Martin Luther King Jr.

How race influences, amplifies backlash against outspoken women

When women break gender norms, the most negative reactions may come from people of the same race.

Examining disparities in education

Scholar Subini Ancy Annamma is studying ways to make education more equitable for historically marginalized students. Annamma’s research examines how schools contribute to the criminalization of Black youths by creating a culture of punishment that penalizes Black children more harshly than their white peers for the same behavior. Her work shows that youth of color are more likely to be closely watched, over-represented in special education, and reported to and arrested by police.

“These are all ways in which schools criminalize Black youth,” she said. “Day after day, these things start to sediment.”

That’s why Annamma has identified opportunities for teachers and administrators to intervene in these unfair practices. Below is some of that research, from Annamma and others.

New ‘Segregation Index’ shows American schools remain highly segregated by race, ethnicity, and economic status

Researchers at Stanford and USC developed a new tool to track neighborhood and school segregation in the U.S.

New evidence shows that school poverty shapes racial achievement gaps

Racial segregation leads to growing achievement gaps – but it does so entirely through differences in school poverty, according to new research from education Professor Sean Reardon, who is launching a new tool to help educators, parents and policymakers examine education trends by race and poverty level nationwide.

School closures intensify gentrification in Black neighborhoods nationwide

An analysis of census and school closure data finds that shuttering schools increases gentrification – but only in predominantly Black communities.

Ninth-grade ethnic studies helped students for years, Stanford researchers find

A new study shows that students assigned to an ethnic studies course had longer-term improvements in attendance and graduation rates.

Teaching about racism

Stanford sociologist Matthew Snipp discusses ways to educate students about race and ethnic relations in America.

Stanford scholar uncovers an early activist’s fight to get Black history into schools

In a new book, Assistant Professor Michael Hines chronicles the efforts of a Chicago schoolteacher in the 1930s who wanted to remedy the portrayal of Black history in textbooks of the time.

How disability intersects with race

Professor Alfredo J. Artiles discusses the complexities in creating inclusive policies for students with disabilities.

Access to program for black male students lowered dropout rates

New research led by Stanford education professor Thomas S. Dee provides the first evidence of effectiveness for a district-wide initiative targeted at black male high school students.

How school systems make criminals of Black youth

Stanford education professor Subini Ancy Annamma talks about the role schools play in creating a culture of punishment against Black students.

Reducing racial disparities in school discipline

Stanford psychologists find that brief exercises early in middle school can improve students’ relationships with their teachers, increase their sense of belonging and reduce teachers’ reports of discipline issues among black and Latino boys.

Science lessons through a different lens

In his new book, Science in the City, Stanford education professor Bryan A. Brown helps bridge the gap between students’ culture and the science classroom.

Teachers more likely to label black students as troublemakers, Stanford research shows

Stanford psychologists Jennifer Eberhardt and Jason Okonofua experimentally examined the psychological processes involved when teachers discipline black students more harshly than white students.

Why we need Black teachers

Travis Bristol, MA '04, talks about what it takes for schools to hire and retain teachers of color.

Understanding racism in the criminal justice system

Research has shown that time and time again, inequality is embedded into all facets of the criminal justice system. From being arrested to being charged, convicted and sentenced, people of color – particularly Black men – are disproportionately targeted by the police.

“So many reforms are needed: police accountability, judicial intervention, reducing prosecutorial power and increasing resources for public defenders are places we can start,” said sociologist Matthew Clair . “But beyond piecemeal reforms, we need to continue having critical conversations about transformation and the role of the courts in bringing about the abolition of police and prisons.”

Clair is one of several Stanford scholars who have examined the intersection of race and the criminal process and offered solutions to end the vicious cycle of racism. Here is some of that work.

Police Facebook posts disproportionately highlight crimes involving Black suspects, study finds

Researchers examined crime-related posts from 14,000 Facebook pages maintained by U.S. law enforcement agencies and found that Facebook users are exposed to posts that overrepresent Black suspects by 25% relative to local arrest rates.

Supporting students involved in the justice system

New data show that a one-page letter asking a teacher to support a youth as they navigate the difficult transition from juvenile detention back to school can reduce the likelihood that the student re-offends.

Race and mass criminalization in the U.S.

Stanford sociologist discusses how race and class inequalities are embedded in the American criminal legal system.

New Stanford research lab explores incarcerated students’ educational paths

Associate Professor Subini Annamma examines the policies and practices that push marginalized students out of school and into prisons.

Derek Chauvin verdict important, but much remains to be done

Stanford scholars Hakeem Jefferson, Robert Weisberg and Matthew Clair weigh in on the Derek Chauvin verdict, emphasizing that while the outcome is important, much work remains to be done to bring about long-lasting justice.

A ‘veil of darkness’ reduces racial bias in traffic stops

After analyzing 95 million traffic stop records, filed by officers with 21 state patrol agencies and 35 municipal police forces from 2011 to 2018, researchers concluded that “police stops and search decisions suffer from persistent racial bias.”

Stanford big data study finds racial disparities in Oakland, Calif., police behavior, offers solutions

Analyzing thousands of data points, the researchers found racial disparities in how Oakland officers treated African Americans on routine traffic and pedestrian stops. They suggest 50 measures to improve police-community relations.

Race and the death penalty

As questions about racial bias in the criminal justice system dominate the headlines, research by Stanford law Professor John J. Donohue III offers insight into one of the most fraught areas: the death penalty.

Diagnosing disparities in health, medicine

The COVID-19 pandemic has disproportionately impacted communities of color and has highlighted the health disparities between Black Americans, whites and other demographic groups.

As Iris Gibbs , professor of radiation oncology and associate dean of MD program admissions, pointed out at an event sponsored by Stanford Medicine: “We need more sustained attention and real action towards eliminating health inequities, educating our entire community and going beyond ‘allyship,’ because that one fizzles out. We really do need people who are truly there all the way.”

Below is some of that research as well as solutions that can address some of the disparities in the American healthcare system.

essay on racial bias

Stanford researchers testing ways to improve clinical trial diversity

The American Heart Association has provided funding to two Stanford Medicine professors to develop ways to diversify enrollment in heart disease clinical trials.

Striking inequalities in maternal and infant health

Research by SIEPR’s Petra Persson and Maya Rossin-Slater finds wealthy Black mothers and infants in the U.S. fare worse than the poorest white mothers and infants.

More racial diversity among physicians would lead to better health among black men

A clinical trial in Oakland by Stanford researchers found that black men are more likely to seek out preventive care after being seen by black doctors compared to non-black doctors.

A better measuring stick: Algorithmic approach to pain diagnosis could eliminate racial bias

Traditional approaches to pain management don’t treat all patients the same. AI could level the playing field.

5 questions: Alice Popejoy on race, ethnicity and ancestry in science

Alice Popejoy, a postdoctoral scholar who studies biomedical data sciences, speaks to the role – and pitfalls – of race, ethnicity and ancestry in research.

Stanford Medicine community calls for action against racial injustice, inequities

The event at Stanford provided a venue for health care workers and students to express their feelings about violence against African Americans and to voice their demands for change.

Racial disparity remains in heart-transplant mortality rates, Stanford study finds

African-American heart transplant patients have had persistently higher mortality rates than white patients, but exactly why still remains a mystery.

Finding the COVID-19 Victims that Big Data Misses

Widely used virus tracking data undercounts older people and people of color. Scholars propose a solution to this demographic bias.

Studying how racial stressors affect mental health

Farzana Saleem, an assistant professor at Stanford Graduate School of Education, is interested in the way Black youth and other young people of color navigate adolescence—and the racial stressors that can make the journey harder.

Infants’ race influences quality of hospital care in California

Disparities exist in how babies of different racial and ethnic origins are treated in California’s neonatal intensive care units, but this could be changed, say Stanford researchers.

Immigrants don’t move state-to-state in search of health benefits

When states expand public health insurance to include low-income, legal immigrants, it does not lead to out-of-state immigrants moving in search of benefits.

Excess mortality rates early in pandemic highest among Blacks

The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic has been starkly uneven across race, ethnicity and geography, according to a new study led by SHP's Maria Polyakova.

Decoding bias in media, technology

Driving Artificial Intelligence are machine learning algorithms, sets of rules that tell a computer how to solve a problem, perform a task and in some cases, predict an outcome. These predictive models are based on massive datasets to recognize certain patterns, which according to communication scholar Angele Christin , sometimes come flawed with human bias . 

“Technology changes things, but perhaps not always as much as we think,” Christin said. “Social context matters a lot in shaping the actual effects of the technological tools. […] So, it’s important to understand that connection between humans and machines.”

Below is some of that research, as well as other ways discrimination unfolds across technology, in the media, and ways to counteract it.

IRS disproportionately audits Black taxpayers

A Stanford collaboration with the Department of the Treasury yields the first direct evidence of differences in audit rates by race.

Automated speech recognition less accurate for blacks

The disparity likely occurs because such technologies are based on machine learning systems that rely heavily on databases of English as spoken by white Americans.

New algorithm trains AI to avoid bad behaviors

Robots, self-driving cars and other intelligent machines could become better-behaved thanks to a new way to help machine learning designers build AI applications with safeguards against specific, undesirable outcomes such as racial and gender bias.

Stanford scholar analyzes responses to algorithms in journalism, criminal justice

In a recent study, assistant professor of communication Angèle Christin finds a gap between intended and actual uses of algorithmic tools in journalism and criminal justice fields.

Move responsibly and think about things

In the course CS 181: Computers, Ethics and Public Policy , Stanford students become computer programmers, policymakers and philosophers to examine the ethical and social impacts of technological innovation.

Homicide victims from Black and Hispanic neighborhoods devalued

Social scientists found that homicide victims killed in Chicago’s predominantly Black and Hispanic neighborhoods received less news coverage than those killed in mostly white neighborhoods.

Algorithms reveal changes in stereotypes

New Stanford research shows that, over the past century, linguistic changes in gender and ethnic stereotypes correlated with major social movements and demographic changes in the U.S. Census data.

AI Index Diversity Report: An Unmoving Needle

Stanford HAI’s 2021 AI Index reveals stalled progress in diversifying AI and a scarcity of the data needed to fix it.

Identifying discrimination in the workplace and economy

From who moves forward in the hiring process to who receives funding from venture capitalists, research has revealed how Blacks and other minority groups are discriminated against in the workplace and economy-at-large. 

“There is not one silver bullet here that you can walk away with. Hiring and retention with respect to employee diversity are complex problems,” said Adina Sterling , associate professor of organizational behavior at the Graduate School of Business (GSB). 

Sterling has offered a few places where employers can expand employee diversity at their companies. For example, she suggests hiring managers track data about their recruitment methods and the pools that result from those efforts, as well as examining who they ultimately hire.

Here is some of that insight.

How To: Use a Scorecard to Evaluate People More Fairly

A written framework is an easy way to hold everyone to the same standard.

Archiving Black histories of Silicon Valley

A new collection at Stanford Libraries will highlight Black Americans who helped transform California’s Silicon Valley region into a hub for innovation, ideas.

Race influences professional investors’ judgments

In their evaluations of high-performing venture capital funds, professional investors rate white-led teams more favorably than they do black-led teams with identical credentials, a new Stanford study led by Jennifer L. Eberhardt finds.

Who moves forward in the hiring process?

People whose employment histories include part-time, temporary help agency or mismatched work can face challenges during the hiring process, according to new research by Stanford sociologist David Pedulla.

How emotions may result in hiring, workplace bias

Stanford study suggests that the emotions American employers are looking for in job candidates may not match up with emotions valued by jobseekers from some cultural backgrounds – potentially leading to hiring bias.

Do VCs really favor white male founders?

A field experiment used fake emails to measure gender and racial bias among startup investors.

Can you spot diversity? (Probably not)

New research shows a “spillover effect” that might be clouding your judgment.

Can job referrals improve employee diversity?

New research looks at how referrals impact promotions of minorities and women.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Published: 18 May 2023

A systemic approach to the psychology of racial bias within individuals and society

  • Allison L. Skinner-Dorkenoo   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-1220-4791 1 ,
  • Meghan George   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-6917-6061 2 ,
  • James E. Wages III   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-4205-9807 3 ,
  • Sirenia Sánchez   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1597-679X 2 &
  • Sylvia P. Perry   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-3435-3125 2 , 4 , 5  

Nature Reviews Psychology volume  2 ,  pages 392–406 ( 2023 ) Cite this article

20k Accesses

17 Citations

53 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Human behaviour
  • Social behaviour

Historically, the field of psychology has focused on racial biases at an individual level, considering the effects of various stimuli on individual racial attitudes and biases. This approach has provided valuable information, but not enough focus has been placed on the systemic nature of racial biases. In this Review, we examine the bidirectional relation between individual-level racial biases and broader societal systems through a systemic lens. We argue that systemic factors operating across levels — from the interpersonal to the cultural — contribute to the production and reinforcement of racial biases in children and adults. We consider the effects of five systemic factors on racial biases in the USA: power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives and values, segregated communities, shared stereotypes and nonverbal messages. We discuss evidence that these factors shape individual-level racial biases, and that individual-level biases shape systems and institutions to reproduce systemic racial biases and inequalities. We conclude with suggestions for interventions that could limit the effects of these influences and discuss future directions for the field.

Similar content being viewed by others

essay on racial bias

Cognitive causes of ‘like me’ race and gender biases in human language production

essay on racial bias

The correct way to test the hypothesis that racial categorization is a byproduct of an evolved alliance-tracking capacity

essay on racial bias

A psycholinguistic study of intergroup bias and its cultural propagation

Introduction.

The field of psychology so far has primarily focused on racial bias at an individual level, centring the effects of various stimuli on the racial biases of individuals 1 , 2 , 3 , 4 , 5 , 6 , 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 . Racial bias refers to favouring or providing preferential treatment to members of one racial group over another. There is no doubt that the approach of focusing on personally held racial prejudices and discrimination has provided valuable information about the psychology of racial biases. However, this approach largely ignores the systemic nature of racial biases and the ways in which racial biases are shaped by the broader cultural systems in which people live 12 , 13 , 14 . For instance, the focus on individual-level biases has contributed to the burgeoning industry of diversity and implicit bias trainings 15 that aim to address problems such as police brutality against Black people 16 . Although potentially useful for changing individual attitudes and/or biases, these interventions seem unlikely to adequately address the underlying causes of bias, which stem from the systems and structures that create and reinforce racial inequality.

The overemphasis on the individual level in research on racial biases seems to have come at the expense of psychological research and theorizing about the impact of broader contextual factors, and how individual-level racial biases reinforce broader systemic patterns of oppression. Models of nested levels of influence in the study of race relations 17 , human development 18 and culture 19 , 20 can be used to consider the relation between racial bias and broader systemic factors (Fig.  1 ). Each level of influence influences the innermost level of individual attitudes, and conversely, individual-level attitudes also influence systems 21 .

figure 1

a , The nested-levels framework focuses on how each systemic level of influence influences individual-level attitudes, and how individual-level attitudes influence systems. The most proximal influences on racial bias are personal and interpersonal experiences, which are nested within communities and institutions that set the local context for interpersonal experiences. Communities are situated within broader cultural contexts that shape the norms, values and beliefs that structure society. At the outermost level are temporal influences, which capture how past manifestations of these systems continue to influence members of society. b , There is a bidirectional influence from each of the systemic levels to the individual level and from the individual level back out to each of the systemic levels, within the five factors discussed in the Review.

Five key systemic factors — power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives and values, segregated communities, shared stereotypes, and nonverbal messages — influence racial bias across the nested levels of this framework. Although they are certainly not the only systemic factors that influence racial biases, we focus on these five because we believe them to be particularly relevant to the development of racial biases in the contemporary USA. At the innermost level are the most proximal influences on individual-level racial bias — personal and interpersonal experiences, such as socialization from caregivers and interracial friendships. These experiences are nested within communities and institutions that set the local context for interpersonal experiences, such as the racial diversity in a school or neighbourhood community. Communities are situated within a broader cultural context that shapes the norms, values, and beliefs that structure society. At the outermost level are temporal influences, which capture how past interpersonal, institutional or community, and societal influences continue to influence members of society throughout their lives. Although the primary focus is on how each level of influence affects individual-level attitudes, the levels also influence one another. For instance, culture can shape organizations and interpersonal experiences within that culture, as well as individual-level biases. Likewise, individual-level racial biases can mould interpersonal experiences, which can shape factors at the organizational and community level.

In this Review, we recognize the bidirectional relation between individual-level racial biases and broader societal systems across levels of influence. In some cases, there is clear evidence of the causal chain from system to individual and from individual to system, whereas in others, there might be evidence of an association, but the direction of influence is unclear. In many cases, individual-level biases and broader societal systems might be mutually reinforcing, but for clarity we take the approach of assessing each direction of influence separately. We examine the role of power and privilege, cultural narratives and values, racial segregation, shared cultural stereotypes and nonverbal signals in racial bias. In each section, we first discuss what is known about the contextual factors that produce and reinforce individual-level biases before highlighting how individual-level biases shape institutions and systems. Although our primary focus is on how these systemic factors influence racial biases, we also use the nested levels of influence framework to examine how individual-level racial biases held by the public can contribute to the reinforcement and perpetuation of systemic oppression at the interpersonal, institutional and/or community, societal and temporal levels. We conclude with implications for interventions aimed at reducing racial bias and discuss future directions for research.

To appropriately analyse the factors that perpetuate racial bias at a systemic level, it is critical to contextualize within culture. We focus on the USA because it is the context that we have the cultural knowledge to discuss and where most research on racial bias development has been conducted. The racial context of the USA is distinct in several ways. Notably, white European colonizers violently stole the land that comprises the USA from indigenous inhabitants across multiple centuries 22 . Furthermore, the enslavement of Black people was legal and common practice for over 200 years in North America 23 , 24 . Citizenship was largely restricted to white people for most of the history of the USA, with full citizenship not open to people of all ethnicities until 1952 (ref. 25 ). Throughout this history, white people have accrued power, wealth, status and numerical majority status through systems that intentionally oppress and marginalize people of colour, including Native Americans, African Americans and members of other ethnic groups. Although modern laws bar racial discrimination, substantial racial inequalities between white people and people of colour persist in the USA, in areas including wealth, education and health 26 , 27 , 28 . Given this history of racism in the USA, the systemic factors that perpetuate racial biases into the present might be somewhat distinct from other contexts. Despite this focus on the USA, this Review could be valuable for understanding similar patterns of bias and oppression outside the USA. Some of these similarities are highlighted in the book Caste: The Origins Of Our Discontents 29 — which draws parallels between the systems of oppression of Black people in the USA, Jewish people in Nazi Germany and Dalit people in India. Although thoroughly analysing such parallels is beyond the scope of this Review, we briefly discuss systemic factors that perpetuate biases based on socially constructed categories — such as race — in other cultural contexts in the concluding section.

Power and privilege disparities

Power and privilege disparities set the initial conditions within which other factors operate. Systemic inequalities in the distribution of power and privilege serve as the societal backdrop in the USA, directly contributing to individual-level racial biases. Many residents of the USA grow up in an environment in which their doctors, lawyers, teachers, government officials, entrepreneurs, and people occupying other respected roles are white 30 , 31 , 32 , 33 . Thus, to these residents, the USA might look like it ‘belongs to’ white people.

Children who are socialized in an unequal society, without systemic explanations for why power and privilege have been concentrated among certain people, often internalize that system 34 . Both children and adults tend to conclude that the way things are structured in society is the way they should be 35 , 36 , 37 , 38 , 39 , 40 , 41 , 42 . For example, experimental evidence suggests that children in the USA tend to prefer people who are relatively more fortunate, even if that good fortune is simply due to luck 43 , and when given the opportunity to rectify existing resource inequalities, they often exacerbate inequalities by giving more to the person who already has more resources 44 . These laboratory findings suggest that when children are socialized in an environment in which people in a particular social group have more resources, they will tend to favour people in that social group and distribute resources in ways that perpetuate resource disparities. Thus, in the USA — where racial wealth disparities are large and growing 28 — systemic inequalities predispose children to infer that white people are better than and deserve to have more than people of colour 45 . Among adults, markers of structural racial inequalities can also predict racial bias. Attending a university with few faculty members of colour, living in an area that has high poverty rates among Black residents and living in a community with low economic mobility all predict heightened implicit bias against Black people among non-Black American residents 7 , 46 . In other words, children and adults are motivated to justify the systems in which they are socialized 41 , 42 .

When progressive changes in society challenge ingrained expectations of inequality, racial biases can be heightened. White residents of the USA who were exposed to information about the increasing racial diversity of the USA subsequently exhibited increased bias favouring white people relative to those who were not exposed to this information 47 , 48 , 49 , 50 . Emphasizing the racially historic milestone of Barack Obama being elected as president of the USA also increased implicit pro-white bias among white American residents, relative to those who were not exposed to this information 50 . This research suggests that racial inequalities at various levels of influence — from local communities to the broader cultural context — can lead people to believe that inequality is natural and justified, and that racial progress that challenges those inequalities might further intensify individual-level racial bias.

Next, we turn to the role of individual-level racial biases in perpetuating systemic racial inequalities. Individual-level racial biases have been argued to impact systemic disparities in power and privilege in a variety of domains, including government representation, population health, education, employment, and immigration 51 . Perhaps the most direct impact of individual-level racial biases on systemic outcomes can be seen in voting behaviour. Greater individual-level anti-Black bias was associated with a lower likelihood of voting for Barack Obama in the 2008 American presidential election and reduced support for his healthcare reform proposal 52 , 53 . With regard to health, white residents of the USA (especially those with higher racial bias) were less supportive of COVID-19 pandemic precautions when they were more aware — based on prior knowledge or experimental exposure to information — that COVID-19 was disproportionately affecting people of colour in the USA 54 , 55 , 56 . Other work has tied racial disparities in health and healthcare access to the average individual-level bias against Black people held by white residents in their county of residence 53 , 57 . In locations where white residents had higher racial biases, Medicaid disability expenditures (which particularly benefit people of colour) were lower, and Black residents had reduced access to healthcare and increased rates of circulatory-disease-related death 53 , 57 . Educational disparities have also been linked to individual-level racial biases. In American counties where the individual-level bias against Black people is stronger, there are larger racial disparities in school disciplinary actions, with Black students being subjected to more suspensions, expulsions, and referrals to law enforcement than white students 58 . These findings suggest that individual-level biases of residents of the USA can influence presidential elections, community health inequalities and school disciplinary actions, reflecting long-term temporal impacts on the privilege and power afforded to people of colour at cultural, community, and interpersonal levels.

In sum, racial disparities in power and privilege have been built into the societal system of the USA, resulting in wide-ranging effects on the life outcomes of residents of the USA. This system also affects the attitudes of those living within the system, leading to expectations of inequality and beliefs that inequality is justified. These beliefs and expectations then motivate the individuals that make up the system to behave in ways that maintain the system of inequality, such that individuals’ attitudes reinforce the system of inequality that produced them.

Cultural narratives and values

The concentration of power and privilege among white people in the USA means that white people largely write the histories, set the norms and define the values of American society. This centring of white people can be seen in historical narratives, cultural products and cultural beliefs, which can all contribute to the development of individual-level racial biases 59 . Below, we discuss the role of each of these aspects of culture in shaping racial biases.

Historical narratives

Historical narratives can play an important part in how people view themselves and others in society 10 . In educational curricula in the USA, national history tends to be taught through a white-affirming lens, such that the attitudes, values and perspectives of white people are implicitly or explicitly justified by the historical narrative 60 , 61 . The perspectives and experiences of people of colour are often omitted from curricula entirely. For instance, despite their many historical contributions to the USA 62 , Asian Americans are vastly underrepresented in American textbooks 63 , 64 , 65 , 66 .

For much of the history of the USA, when people of colour were discussed in textbooks, they were described with derogatory and dehumanizing stereotypes that justified their marginalization 60 , 67 . Moreover, egregious acts perpetrated by white people have often been presented in a sanitized way that minimizes, glosses over, and justifies them 68 , 69 , 70 . As an example, ‘manifest destiny’ (the worldview that white people were destined to expand their territory to the west coast of North America) is often presented uncritically as a justification for atrocities that white people committed against the original Native American and Mexican inhabitants of the continent 60 , 71 . Similarly, Confederate symbols — which celebrate the southern American states that went to war with northern American states to maintain the institution of slavery — are argued to be a race-neutral representation of Southern pride in some textbooks and by some modern pundits and continue to be displayed at some courthouses 72 , 73 , 74 . The way history is usually portrayed in American society therefore obscures the relation between contemporary systems and racial injustices of the past 61 . Some states have even created laws explicitly barring the teaching of critical history related to race 75 .

The way history is presented in society shapes individual-level attitudes about race and racism. School curricula can be vital contributors to ethnocentric biases in childhood 76 . Furthermore, adults who have less knowledge of the racial injustices of the past tend to be less aware of present-day racism 77 . Exposure to historical narratives that centre white people and glorify the nation reduce awareness of racial injustices among school children, college students, and adult museum visitors 70 , 78 . Furthermore, experimental evidence suggests that exposure to the Confederate flag (versus no exposure to the flag) can increase racial biases and promote racial injustice 79 . Thus, how history is portrayed at a cultural level, in textbooks, and in community schools and institutions (such as museums and memorials) has the potential to influence individual-level racial biases.

Individuals receive and simultaneously reproduce and uphold these historical narratives. For instance, it was argued in 1963 that the individual-level racial biases of historians were to blame for the history of Native Americans being oversimplified, mischaracterized and/or overlooked entirely 80 . Although there have been changes to the framing of the history of the USA over the intervening 60 years, many of the issues identified persist to this day 81 . For example, Christopher Columbus is still often credited with and praised for ‘discovering’ North America, even though it was already inhabited by thriving interconnected societies of millions of people 22 .

The effects of individual-level biases can also be seen at the community and interpersonal levels. The same history and culture can be represented differently depending on who is curating and constructing the representation 78 , 82 . As an example, students who reported that being white was central to their identity reported more negative attitudes towards Black History Month representations that were curated and displayed in schools in which the majority of students were Black (versus those that were curated and displayed in schools in which the majority of students were white) 70 . This finding is particularly meaningful because Black History Month representations in schools with a majority of Black students were generally more supportive of anti-racism than those in schools with a majority of white students. Another study provided evidence that patrons of the Ellis Island Immigration Museum tended to identify exhibits that painted the USA in a positive light as more important than exhibits highlighting historical injustices 78 . Patrons who reported more assimilationist attitudes — such as believing that to ‘be truly American’ means speaking English — particularly disliked the exhibits highlighting historical injustices. All of these findings converge to suggest that individual-level biases shape the way people in the USA think about and portray history at personal, community, cultural and temporal levels.

Cultural products

At a societal level, the cultural products — such as art forms, varieties of dress and appearance, and styles of speech — of white people tend to be the most highly regarded. The art, music, and dance that are most culturally valued in the USA are rooted in white European traditions 83 , 84 . For instance, professional dance schools largely focus on ballet and modern dance 85 and music departments primarily emphasize classical music education — marginalizing the study of dance and music traditions developed by artists of colour 86 , 87 . Expectations for appropriate dress and communication are also centred around norms established by white Americans. In some cases, schools 88 and workplaces 89 have created policies and municipalities have passed laws 90 prohibiting styles of dress that are culturally linked to people of colour, such as durags, hijabs, and sagging pants. Even when these marginalized styles are not explicitly banned, there might be added scrutiny of individuals who wear them 91 . As an example, Black women are often expected to conform to white femininity norms by straightening their hair to be perceived as professional 90 , 92 , 93 . Furthermore, grammatical rules and standard linguistic styles in the USA are based on the language practices of white Americans 94 and deviations from these norms — such as use of African American Vernacular English (AAVE) — are often cited as evidence of inferiority 95 , 96 , 97 , 98 . Although there can be conditional acceptance and appropriation of elements of the cultures of people of colour such as styles of dress 99 , the typical situation is the prioritization of cultural products of white people.

In summary, the cultural products of people of colour are devalued and stigmatized at a societal level, which results in individual-level biases against those who use and produce these cultural products. This contribution to individual-level racial bias is particularly insidious because it creates conditions under which people can obliviously perpetuate racial biases, believing their bias to be against the cultural products they perceive as inferior rather than the people associated with the products. In one hypothetical scenario at an interpersonal level, parents might pass along individual-level racial biases to their children when parents conclude that the classmate with dreadlocked hair (a hairstyle with African roots that is common among Black Americans) looks like a troublemaker. In this way, Americans can look down on Black people who engage with Black cultural products — like listening to hip hop music, having dreadlocks or using AAVE — while still believing themselves to be ‘not racist’. Devaluing Black cultural products perpetuates racism, because doing so suggests that Black culture is inferior to white culture.

Individual-level biases can also feed back to contribute to the reproduction of biased cultural products. White people are more likely than people of other races to rise to positions of economic power and influence 100 , and therefore to enact their individual biases and serve as gatekeepers for the promotion of certain cultural products over others. At the most extreme, individual-level biases of white people in power have resulted in cultural genocide, such as through Native American boarding schools, which systematically destroyed Native American families and communities 101 . The white people who created and promoted Native American boarding schools argued that Native American communities were dangerous and that Native children needed to be rescued by ‘good Christians’ 102 . These arguments were consistent with widespread beliefs about the cultural inferiority of Native Americans, which allowed racist policies, such as those forcing all Native American children to live in boarding schools that barred them from practising their culture, seem acceptable.

Evidence of the effects of widely held individual-level white-centric biases can also be observed on a cultural level in academic fields, influencing their methods, standards, and knowledge bases (for example, in mathematics 103 , 104 , psychology 105 , and written composition 87 , 94 ). As an example, individual-level biases towards white cultural products arguably led to the development of writing standards that privilege white styles of discourse 106 . From a historical perspective, although Native Americans had a diverse array of numeric systems that were still in use at the turn of the twentieth century 107 , individual-level biases towards white people and culture led ‘Western mathematics’ to be established as the standard for mathematics education in the USA, marginalizing indigenous knowledge 103 . The effect of individual-level biases among key decision-makers can also be seen in policy decisions, such as whose cultural holidays are officially recognized as public holidays 108 and whose cultural knowledge frames the questions used in standardized testing 109 , 110 , 111 .

Cultural beliefs

Widely held cultural beliefs and philosophies can shape the way in which individuals make sense of society, contributing to individual-level racial biases. In the USA, the ‘American dream’ — which asserts that anyone can achieve success if they are willing to put in the work — is a dominant philosophy in government, education and the media. The belief that people who merit success will ultimately achieve it can serve as a beacon of hope for those who are striving to raise their social status, while also justifying to people with the highest status that they have rightfully earned their positions. Thus, promoting the American dream reinforces tendencies for people to justify and reinforce the systems of inequality in which they were socialized 41 , 42 , 112 , 113 . Indeed, priming American residents with messages promoting meritocracy (versus other types of messages) can reduce recognition of unearned privilege 114 and increase blame placed on people disadvantaged by societal systems 115 . Given the many racial inequalities in American society, teaching children that people who deserve success achieve it conveys the implicit message that most people of colour do not merit the status and success that white people enjoy in the USA 45 — imparting individual-level racial bias.

Racially colourblind ideology (the idea of disregarding the issue of race) is another cultural philosophy that can contribute to the perpetuation of individual-level racial biases. Polling data from 2020 indicated that approximately 40% of American residents believed that paying less attention to race would improve racial inequalities in society 116 . Yet, evidence suggests that racially colourblind messages provide a way for racially biased messages to discreetly influence public attitudes. For example, white Americans are much more likely to be persuaded to adopt policy positions through subtle racial appeals referring to Black people — such as references to the ‘inner city’ or ‘culture of poverty’ — than they are to be persuaded by explicit racial appeals that refer to Black people as lazy and uneducated welfare recipients 117 , 118 . Politicians have deliberately used these ‘dog whistles’ to capitalize on racial stereotypes and gain public support for racist political agendas 119 , 120 . Even ostensibly well intentioned colourblind attitudes and policies can perpetuate racial biases 121 . For instance, exposing children to the racially colourblind mindset, compared to a diversity mindset, reduced their ability to identify racially biased incidents and appropriately report them to teachers 122 . Promoting racial colourblindness at a societal, institutional or even an interpersonal level can reduce individual-level awareness of systemic racism and increase susceptibility to racially biased messages.

In the reverse direction, individual-level racial biases can also influence cultural beliefs. Americans with higher levels of racial bias tend to report a stronger belief in meritocracy 123 , 124 . Moreover, young, white adults with relatively high socio-economic status are more likely than people of other races and lower economic status to believe that the USA is a meritocracy 125 . Individual-level racial biases are also associated with more support for racially colourblind sentiments, such as ‘society would be better off if we all stopped talking about race’ 126 , 127 . Some psychologists have theorized that racial biases can motivate racially colourblind perspectives that help to maintain ignorance of racial injustices 128 . Racially colourblind ideology has also served to perpetuate systemic racism through government policies (Box  1 ). Taken together, individual-level racial biases have been associated with cultural philosophies that obscure and reinforce racial inequalities across societal levels.

Altogether, historical narratives that exclude people of colour and downplay the history of racism in the USA, cultural norms that devalue the cultural products of people of colour, and cultural beliefs that obscure systems of inequality contribute to the development and maintenance of individual-level racial biases. Once individual-level racial biases have been established, these attitudes cumulatively shape how history is told, what cultural knowledge and products make up the mainstream, and what cultural beliefs are promoted.

Box 1 Colourblind ideology

In the USA, there are numerous examples of societal-level colourblind racial ideology being used to uphold systemic racial oppression. For instance, following the Brown v. Board of Education Supreme Court ruling (1954), separate schools for Black and white students were ruled unconstitutional. Consequently, school boards in multiple American states continued to segregate students by race but claimed that school assignments were now based on ‘fit’ and ‘ability’, rather than race 264 . In this example, the colourblind ideology of assignment by ability was a means of perpetuating systemic oppression while also obscuring systemic racism and reinforcing racist stereotypes about intellectual abilities. A similar series of events took place following the Batson v. Kentucky (1986) Supreme Court decision, which ruled that potential jurors could not be removed on the basis of their race. Following the decision, attorneys continued to remove Black potential jurors, but now provided a colourblind rationale 265 , 266 . Jurors can be struck for such arbitrary reasons as wearing a hat, being unemployed, or the prosecutor simply having a bad feeling about them 266 .

Perhaps the greatest effect of the use of colourblind ideology emerged as part of the so-called War on Drugs, a federal campaign in the USA that began in 1971 to crack down on the possession and sale of illegal drugs. One of its most notorious policies set a penalty 100 times higher for possession of substances that were disproportionately used by Black American residents than for substances disproportionately used by white American residents 210 . The War on Drugs has been credited with the massive increase in the prison population and dramatic growth of racial disparities in incarceration rates in the USA during the late twentieth century 267 . As of 2021, Black Americans were incarcerated at roughly five times the rate of white Americans 268 , 269 . Overall, racially colourblind ideology has allowed systemic policies that are ostensibly race-neutral to remain racially biased in effect.

Segregated communities

Segregation in neighbourhoods, workplaces, and classrooms is another systemic influence on individual-level racial bias. Segregation in the USA is often a result of the power and privilege disparities and cultural narratives discussed above. For instance, ‘redlining’ was a mid-twentieth-century policy in which neighbourhoods were graded (from ‘desirable’ to ‘hazardous’) according to their supposed risk of decreasing in value. Neighbourhoods populated by people of colour were assigned the lowest scores (demarcated by a red border on a map), which both reduced the value of these homes and prevented homebuyers from accessing federally backed and insured loans 129 , 130 . The enduring impact of this policy is that families of colour have not been able to build the generational wealth through homeownership that white people have 28 , 131 . To this day, homeowners in neighbourhoods that were deemed hazardous have dramatically less home equity than homeowners in areas that were deemed desirable 132 . Thus, residents of the USA see that nice homes and other symbols of wealth are associated with white people, promoting and reinforcing individual-level pro-white biases 130 .

The echoes of this policy also reinforce residential segregation because homebuyers with adequate means (who are more likely to be white) are motivated to avoid redlined neighbourhoods, where homes tend to be devalued and appreciate far less over time 132 . Thus, racial segregation remains commonplace in the USA. For instance, the average white American lives in a neighbourhood whose residents are 75% white 133 . Similarly, workplace racial segregation was higher in the 2010s than it was in the 1980s and 1990s 134 . Although public school segregation was ruled unconstitutional in 1954 (ref. 135 ), the last school district only fully integrated in 2017 (ref. 136 ). Even within racially integrated schools, a biased system of sorting students into different and unequal course tracks results in overrepresentation of white students in the more advanced and better resourced tracks 137 .

Ongoing segregation also limits intergroup contact 138 — contact between people belonging to different racial or ethnic groups — one of the most reliable and best studied predictors of reduced individual-level bias 139 . Close contact between groups has been argued to reduce prejudice when those groups share common goals, hold equal status, cooperate with each other and are supported by the broader societal system 138 . Systemic racism in the USA reduces the likelihood of these conditions being met, but the bulk of the evidence indicates that even when optimal conditions are not met, positive intergroup contact reduces prejudice 140 , 141 , 142 . Thus, the fact that so many social environments — including communities, institutions and interpersonal experiences — in the USA remain racially segregated probably contributes to the persistence of individual-level racial biases.

Limited opportunities for close contact might be particularly detrimental when groups share the same geographic space but lack close contact with one another. A 2015 study found that racial biases among white residents of the USA were highest in states where the Black population was largest 143 . A follow-up study examined how this pattern relates to intergroup contact, finding that living in a state with more Black residents was only associated with increased racial bias among white residents who had limited close contact with Black people 144 . Thus, when white people live alongside people of colour in their communities without forming close relationships, racial bias might increase. As such, racial segregation in racially diverse regions and states seems particularly likely to engender individual-level racial biases because intergroup exposure can elicit group threat 145 , without the psychological benefits provided by meaningful emotional connections developed through close intergroup contact.

Individual-level racial biases can also reinforce segregated communities and systems. Black communities in the USA are stereotyped as being impoverished, crime-ridden, rundown, dangerous, dirty, and ‘ghetto’ 146 , 147 . If key decision-makers or a critical mass of members of the public hold these biases, devaluation of physical spaces associated with people of colour (such as schools and neighbourhoods) can result 148 , 149 , 150 , 151 . There is experimental evidence that homes in predominantly Black neighbourhoods and homes owned by Black (versus white) people tend to be devalued. When white residents of the USA were asked to assign value to a home, they thought the home was worth less if an image of a Black (relative to white) family appeared in front of the home, even though all other factors were the same 146 . The same patterns emerge in actual housing data 152 . Schools in predominantly Black neighbourhoods are undervalued, receiving less funding per student than schools in predominantly white neighbourhoods 153 .

This disregard for neighbourhoods populated by people of colour can also be seen in decisions about infrastructure, such as where to place hazardous waste dumps and what communities to displace when new amenities such as freeways and railroads are introduced into communities. Eminent domain — a legal power that forces private citizens to sell land to the government for public projects —is more likely to be used in communities of colour 154 , 155 and has myriad negative consequences, including loss of wealth and disruption of community ties and stability 156 , 157 . Experimental evidence has indicated that white Americans are less likely to oppose placing a hypothetical chemical plant next to a Black neighbourhood than next to a white neighbourhood 146 . Real-world data are consistent with this hypothetical scenario: the highest polluting industries and sites for toxic waste disposal in the USA tend to be in areas with large populations of Black people 158 , 159 . By contrast, experimental evidence indicates that landmarks that are of interest to white people (such as their workplaces, schools, pools, golf clubs and tennis clubs) tend to be placed relatively far from communities of colour 160 . Furthermore, analyses of existing institutional policies show that recreation facilities that are closer to communities of colour have more exclusionary barriers — such as fees or dress codes 160 , 161 . The history of practices of this kind explain the relatively low swimming rates among Black Americans and dramatically higher drowning rates among Black (relative to white) American residents 162 , 163 , 164 .

Overall, individual-level racial biases can contribute to the devaluation and negative stereotyping of physical spaces occupied by people of colour, which make those spaces less desirable to white people—reinforcing racial segregation. These racial biases might ultimately contribute to a host of systemic and structural racial inequalities within domains ranging from health and wealth to education, which adversely impact people of colour for generations.

Shared stereotypes

Shared stereotypes can spread cultural narratives and justify oppressive systems. Propaganda campaigns have been used to spread stereotypes about people of colour throughout the history of the USA. For instance, nineteenth-century media stereotypes of men of Chinese descent as weak and effeminate 165 , 166 were used to marginalize these men, painting them as poorly suited for traditionally masculine jobs and undesirable as husbands 167 . Propaganda about intellect, criminality, and sexuality has been used by the media, politicians, industries, and scientists to exercise social control over Black Americans and justify their enslavement and subordination for centuries 23 , 168 , 169 (and see a preprint 170 ).

Stereotypical representations of people of colour continue in the contemporary media 34 . On television, people of colour tend to be depicted in negative and stereotypical ways 171 , 172 , 173 , as outside mainstream contemporary society 174 or not represented at all 175 . News coverage also tends to depict people of colour in a negative light 176 , 177 , 178 and Black criminal suspects are often overrepresented in news media — perpetuating stereotypes that Black people are ‘criminal’ and ‘reckless’ 179 , 180 . These stereotypes represent only a few of the many ways people of colour are negatively stereotyped in the USA. Meanwhile, white people are overrepresented in media coverage of crime victims 174 , 181 , 182 .

Societal stereotypes are also shared in more subtle ways. Shared cultural stereotypes can develop through the selection and transmission of specific pieces of information about social groups 183 , 184 and consequently stereotypes that have no connection to actual traits can form and spread widely. A variety of linguistic biases have been identified as ways in which stereotypes are subtly spread through social groups 185 , 186 , 187 . For instance, more specific and trivial wording — such as ‘stole a pack of gum’ — might be used to describe the theft behaviours of white people, whereas more abstract and dramatic terms — such as ‘shoplifted’ — might be used for Black people 188 . There is also a noted tendency for media to use the passive voice when reporting on structural and systemic harms perpetrated against people of colour — reporting, for instance, that ‘Black Lives Matter protesters were teargassed’ as opposed to the more active phrasing ‘police teargassed Black Lives Matter protesters’ 189 . Knowledge of these shared stereotypes can lead to individual-level biases 190 , 191 . White residents of the USA with more exposure to stereotypical portrayals of people of colour tend to report more racial-stereotype-consistent perceptions 177 , 192 , 193 and greater bias against people of colour 175 . More time spent watching local news — often containing stereotypical representations of people of colour — has been associated with increased bias against members of marginalized groups 194 , 195 , 196 . Thus, shared stereotypes at several of the nested levels of influence, from interpersonal interactions to the societal level through media, can shape individual-level racial biases.

Individual-level racial biases also have the potential to shape shared stereotypes at various levels. For example, a white individual’s racial biases might lead them to perceive a Black man walking in their neighbourhood as suspicious. The white individual might share their concerns with their neighbours (in person or through online message boards), spreading associations between Black people and criminality. After being primed by this message, another neighbour might call the police on a Black man in the neighbourhood. Neighbours who see the police treating the man as a suspect might infer that he is a criminal, further reinforcing inaccurate stereotypes of Black people as criminals. Given widespread anti-Black biases in the USA 197 and known tendencies to associate Black people with crime 198 , 199 , scenarios like this are not unlikely. Indeed, Black men in the USA are more likely than white men to be stopped, searched, handcuffed, and arrested by police 200 , 201 , 202 , 203 . Racial biases can also result in much graver outcomes. White American residents with stronger anti-Black biases also tend to be stronger supporters of gun rights 204 , 205 (though less so if they are primed to think about Black gun owners 206 , 207 ), and in another study white residents of the USA were less concerned about gun deaths when the victims are Black than when they are white 208 . Moreover, where residents had greater implicit biases against Black people, there was more disproportionate police lethal force directed towards Black residents 209 .

The fact that most people in the USA have individual-level racial biases 144 also limits support for policy change and allows systemic racial inequalities in the criminal justice system to be overlooked 8 , 210 , 211 . Perceiving Black men as threatening is associated with increased fear of crime and reduced support for system-level reform, such as police body cameras and matching the demographics of a police force to their community 212 . Individual-level racial bias might also lead the public to reify racial inequalities when faced with evidence of racial disparities in the American criminal justice system 213 , 214 . For instance, Americans who were told about more extreme racial disparities in the prison population of the USA were less willing to support system-level reforms 215 . Overall, individual-level racial biases can lead to behaviours that transmit racial stereotypes at interpersonal and community levels, which ultimately have the potential to reinforce systemic racial inequalities in communities and in broader society with long-term ramifications.

Nonverbal messages

People are constantly exposed to nonverbal messages, such as facial expressions and body language, in contexts from workplaces and schools 216 to depictions in the media 217 . These nonverbal signals can often convey societal messages, such as racial biases and cultural messages about race. For example, for most of the history of the USA, especially in the southern states, Black people were expected to demean themselves to white people through nonverbal behaviours, for instance by stepping off the sidewalk and removing their hats when a white person passed by 218 . These nonverbal signals were choreographed to communicate white superiority, and failure to adhere to this racial choreography could have violent — even deadly — consequences for Black people 24 , 218 . These racialized expectations for respect persist. For instance, analyses of recorded police interactions with the public from within the past five years indicated that officers spoke less respectfully and had less friendliness and respect in their tone of voice when interacting with Black men than with white men 219 , 220 .

Nonverbal racial biases have also been documented in American media. For example, white characters on primetime television often receive more warm, positive nonverbal signals, such as smiles, than do Black characters 217 . Exposure to nonverbal biases of this kind can influence the attitudes of children and adults 216 , 221 , 222 , 223 , increasing negative attitudes towards Black people 217 , 224 , 225 , 226 . Even exposure to nonverbal bias towards a single member of a group has the potential to produce group biases. For instance, adults and children who observed a series of brief interactions in which one person systematically received more warm, friendly nonverbal signals than another person subsequently favoured the person who received the more positive nonverbal signals, and also favoured others of their fictitious nationality 222 , 227 . Thus, if a sufficient number of people in a community or society — especially people with social influence, such as teachers or community leaders — display nonverbal biases favouring white people, other individuals in that context will also develop the same biases. For instance, if children observe a systematic pattern of white role models seeming more anxious when passing Black people on the street, that will shape their attitudes and emotions about who they feel safe around.

Individual-level biases can also shape people, communities, and culture. Individuals with more racially biased attitudes tend to show more racially biased nonverbal behaviour in interracial interactions 228 , 229 , 230 , 231 . Thus, individual-level attitudes seem to shape nonverbal behaviour in interpersonal interactions. Exposure to this nonverbal manifestation of individual-level bias can also influence the behaviours of others. Children who are exposed to biased nonverbal signals in social interactions often adopt the biased behaviours they observe 221 , 222 . For instance, preschool children who observed a member of one group receive more nonverbal warmth than a member of another group were subsequently more likely to choose their playmates from the former group 222 . Furthermore, 40% of preschool children who were exposed to biased nonverbal signals in a study mimicked the biased nonverbal signals they observed 232 .

If biased nonverbal signals and the attitudes they convey are easily transmitted and adopted by an individual, it follows that they will propagate beyond the individual to their social network. Thus, nonverbal biases can be thought of as an expanding system of transmission wherein exposure to nonverbal biases contributes to the development of racially biased attitudes; those attitudes then manifest as biased nonverbal signals, which others within the social network will then see, leading them to develop biases, and so on 233 .

The tendency to transmit racial biases nonverbally might be exacerbated by the fact that people tend to justify the nonverbal biases they observe. Even when the targets of (positively or negatively) biased nonverbal signals displayed identical behaviours, observers were more likely to report that the targets’ behaviour (57%), rather than how the target was treated (30%), influenced their attitudes towards the target 223 , 227 . In other words, when the only thing that varied was how targets were treated by others, observers still justified their own attitudes by attributing them to the targets’ actions.

Taken together, if the majority of individuals making up a social system hold the same individual racial biases, repeated exposure and reinforcement of those racial biases through nonverbal behaviours can potentially shape attitudes and reinforce systemic oppression at interpersonal, institutional (classrooms) and societal (media) levels. Moreover, when nonverbal messages propagate racial biases to children, they spread racial biases temporally, to future generations.

Implications for psychological interventions

Given our thesis that systemic factors strongly contribute to the development and perpetuation of racist attitudes and beliefs, the importance of changing these systemic factors (including systems, policies, and practices) cannot be overstated. However, we focus our discussion here on psychological interventions (Table  1 ). We frame our discussion of interventions in terms of what can be done at a psychological level to interrupt or limit the effects of the systemic influences and feedback cycles identified above.

One potential lever with which to reduce the spread of individual racial biases is for white parents to teach their children directly about the importance of race and racism (as parents of colour do), a process called racial socialization. With the proliferation of colourblind racial ideology, many white American residents believe that disregarding race would help to minimize racism 234 , and white parents have feared that acknowledging race could facilitate the development of racial biases in their children 234 , 235 . Although some scholars have cautioned that there might be pitfalls to white parents talking to their children about race and racism, such as ill-equipped parents being ineffectual or producing counterproductive effects 236 , many scholars agree that racial socialization in white American families is a step in the right direction 139 , 237 , 238 , 239 .

Parental racial socialization has the potential to provide children with a new lens through which they can interpret the societal system in which they are immersed and help them to recognize systemic racism 240 , 241 . Consequently, it can provide a counternarrative to the systemic factors reviewed here that perpetuate systemic racism. Racial socialization of children can prevent them from accepting, internalizing, and justifying the existing system of power and privilege, and instead enable them to see it as unjust and in need of reform. Initial evidence suggests that racial socialization in white American families can reduce racial biases among children 239 , 242 , 243 . In a preprint that has not yet undergone peer review, 8–12-year-old white American children who engaged in a semi-structured conversation about race and interpersonal racism with a parent showed a statistically significant decrease in implicit anti-Black racial bias from after the conversation compared to before 239 . As noted in the section on segregation, direct interracial contact — which is known to reduce racial biases 138 , 139 , 140 , 141 , 142 — is often limited by social and residential segregation. Thus, when direct contact with children of colour is not an option, parents can use books, films and other media to help children to understand the varied lived experiences of children in other racial and ethnic groups and ultimately to reduce their prejudices 244 , 245 , 246 , 247 , 248 , 249 , 250 .

Alongside the socialization of children, another valuable intervention effort is to develop bias awareness among adults. Greater awareness of one’s own biases is associated with less racially biased attitudes and intentions to engage in less racially biased behaviour 251 , 252 , 253 . White Americans who are more aware of their own racial biases are more willing to accept feedback on their racial biases and are more likely to detect evidence of subtle racial biases in themselves and others 253 . Thus, people who are more bias-aware might also be more likely to recognize systemic racial biases. When white Americans are presented with blatantly racist messages and are aware of the bias in the message, they tend to resist the influence of the message 117 , 254 , 255 . As such, helping people to recognize the subtle biases communicated through cultural narratives, nonverbal signals, and shared stereotypes across various levels of influence — such as racial ‘dog whistles’ — could reduce the effects of these biases. Consistent with the notion that racial bias awareness increases awareness of systemic racism, white American parents with greater bias awareness indicated that they were more likely to discuss racial current events reflective of systemic racism with their 8–12-year-old children 256 .

Our final intervention recommendation is education about the history and systems that led to current racial injustices and inequalities. People who are more knowledgeable about the racial injustices of the past tend to be more aware of the systemic racism that persists in the present 77 , 257 . There is also growing evidence that educating children and adults about historical racial injustices can improve racial attitudes, increase empathy, and raise awareness of systemic racism 258 , 259 , 260 . For instance, white American children who learned about the accomplishments of Black historical figures — and the systemic and interpersonal racism they faced — showed less racial stereotyping and more positive attitudes towards Black people, and placed greater value on racial fairness than children who learned only about the historical figures’ accomplishments 259 . A preprint that has not yet undergone peer review also suggests that simply informing people of a group’s history of being treated unjustly can help to buffer the spread of nonverbal biases about that group 227 . Historical education can challenge people’s tendencies to infer that power and privilege disparities are justified and further call into question the cultural narratives that elevate white people and white culture.

These recommendations for intervention are overlapping and complementary. It is important to identify multiple ways to intervene in the nested levels of influence that shape attitudes and the ways that attitudes contribute to the perpetuation of systemic issues. By encouraging racial socialization, bias awareness, and accurate historical education, Americans can make new meaning of societal systems, understand better how the country got here, and begin to chip away at the racism so deeply ingrained in their society. These interventions would be particularly effective if implemented at a systemic level, for example, by revising the teaching of history and incorporating critical racial education into public school curricula.

Summary and future directions

In this Review, we approached the question of how racial biases develop, observing through a systemic lens how influences at the interpersonal, community, societal and temporal levels interact with racial biases. We considered the effects of five cross-cutting systemic factors on racial biases: unequal distributions of power and privilege, historical narratives and cultural products, racial segregation, shared cultural stereotypes, and nonverbal signals. We provided evidence that each of these five systemic factors shapes individual-level racial biases, and that individual-level racial biases shape systems and inequalities at each of the nested levels. Similar arguments about individuals and systems mutually reinforcing one another have been made in the context of organizations and the workplace 11 . We also reviewed interventions that could buffer the effects of the systemic influences we identified. Specifically, we reviewed the literature on racial socialization in white families, racial bias awareness and historical education — highlighting the promise of each.

The psychological study of racial attitudes has largely focused on individual-level biases, dedicating relatively little research to the systemic forces that shape racial biases. However, psychological scholars should seek opportunities to understand better how systems influence racial biases. Given the public availability of large-scale surveys and polling data indexing racial attitudes, there is great potential for scholars to use changes in state-level policies or practices as opportunities to examine the influence of systemic factors on racial attitudes.

Although many of the processes we describe here might generalize to other cultural and national contexts, the most relevant factors and how they manifest might vary considerably across contexts. In Box  2 , we briefly discuss literature from non-USA contexts and the ways in which it converges and diverges from patterns observed in the USA. However, we by no means provide a comprehensive review. Ultimately, much more research on prejudice and racism needs to be conducted outside the USA and other nations with a majority of white residents 261 , 262 , 263 . Research across a variety of cultural contexts is essential to the development of a comprehensive understanding of the societal systems that produce and reinforce racial (and other) biases, and the psychological processes that contribute to the reproduction of societal inequalities.

Although there is a growing body of literature tying systemic influences to individual-level biases, the literature in this area is still relatively sparse. For instance, it has yet to be experimentally tested how exposure to cultural products influences racial attitudes. There is also much work to be done regarding the development of interventions. For instance, it is important to identify optimal strategies for approaching racial socialization in white families and determining its impact on children’s racial attitudes. As an example, it is unknown whether the effects of parental racial socialization vary as a function of whether it involves teaching about interpersonal versus systemic racism. There is also still much to be learned about how often these conversations should take place and how long their effects persist. Racial bias awareness seems to be helpful for recognizing interpersonal biases, but it remains to be seen whether racial bias awareness facilitates recognition of systemic racism. Perhaps most importantly, it is unclear whether interventions aimed at developing racial bias awareness can be effective. We posit that interventions that integrate efforts to increase bias awareness, conversations about systemic and interpersonal racism, and education about historical injustices would pack the most powerful punch — but work that empirically tests the efficacy of such interventions is still needed. Moreover, applied interventions that examine how these factors operate in systemic contexts — such as schools — will be an important future step.

To fully interrupt the processes we have described, interventions will need to target organizations, policies, institutions and systems, as well as how individuals interact with each of these areas of influence. For interventions to truly be successful, they will need to cut across the nested levels of influence, lest any progress at one level be washed out by the continuing biases present at other levels. Large systemic changes of this kind require buy-in from key stakeholders and the public, and because change can take time, developing strategies to target how the public understands and makes sense of systems — and whether they are seen as just — might be a key step on the path towards an equitable society.

Box 2 Systemic bias outside the USA

Many of the factors reviewed here also apply in other cultural contexts. For instance, power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives, segregation and shared stereotypes have been identified as contributors to individual-level racial biases in South Africa 270 , 271 . Power and privilege disparities in South Africa are stark; in 2017 white people held 67% of the top management positions but accounted for less than 8% of the country’s population 272 . Such dramatic status disparities have been identified as a key contributor to racial biases among South African children, across races 273 . Furthermore, media sources in South Africa have been criticized for promoting societal narratives that justify racial oppression and reinforce negative stereotypes about Black South Africans 271 .

Turning to a different cultural context, the extreme power and privilege disparities inherent in the statuses assigned to groups within the Hindu caste system have been identified as important contributors to the perpetuation of caste-based biases among Indian children 274 . Likewise, power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives, segregation, shared stereotypes and nonverbal biases have been identified as contributors to Israeli Jewish children’s biases against Arab people 275 , 276 , 277 , 278 , 279 .

Some aspects of social bias development are not shared across cultures. For instance, in the contemporary USA, norms of egalitarianism and laws prohibiting discrimination limit the extent to which members of society are explicitly socialized to favour white people and disparage people of colour. Thus, racial biases tend to be transmitted in implicit, coded and plausibly deniable ways. In cultural contexts in which openly expressing biased and discriminatory views is socially acceptable, as was the case in earlier periods of American history, more explicit socialization of bias often takes place. For instance, within the Jewish population in Israel, people tend to be relatively accepting of anti-Arab biases, so children might be deliberately socialized to avoid and distrust Arab people 276 , 277 , 279 .

Adams, G., Edkins, V., Lacka, D., Pickett, K. M. & Cheryan, S. Teaching about racism: pernicious implications of the standard portrayal. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 30 , 349–361 (2008).

Article   Google Scholar  

Buchanan, N. T., Perez, M., Prinstein, M. J. & Thurston, I. B. Upending racism in psychological science: strategies to change how science is conducted, reported, reviewed & disseminated. Am. Psychol. 76 , 1097–1112 (2021).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Malherbe, N., Ratele, K., Adams, G., Reddy, G. & Suffla, S. A decolonial Africa(n)-centered psychology of antiracism. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 25 , 437–450 (2021).

Payne, B. K. & Hannay, J. W. Implicit bias reflects systemic racism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25 , 927–936 (2021). This paper discusses evidence that implicit biases reflect systemic conditions and advocates for a shift towards broader systems in the study of racial bias.

Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A. & Lundberg, K. B. The bias of crowds: how implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychol. Inq. 28 , 233–248 (2017).

Payne, B. K. & Vuletich, H. A. Policy insights from advances in implicit bias research. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 5 , 49–56 (2018).

Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A. & Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L. Historical roots of implicit bias in slavery. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 116 , 11693 (2019).

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Rucker, J. M. & Richeson, J. A. Toward an understanding of structural racism: implications for criminal justice. Science 374 , 286–290 (2021).

Salter, P. & Adams, G. Toward a critical race psychology. Soc. Person. Psychol. Compass 7 , 781–793 (2013).

Salter, P. S., Adams, G. & Perez, M. J. Racism in the structure of everyday worlds: a cultural-psychological perspective. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27 , 150–155 (2018). This paper uses a cultural-psychological perspective to examine how racism is embedded in and reproduced by culture.

Stephens, N. M., Rivera, L. A. & Townsend, S. S. M. The cycle of workplace bias and how to interrupt it. Res. Organ. Behav. 40 , 100137 (2020).

Google Scholar  

Dobbin, F. & Kalev, A. Why doesn’t diversity training work? The challenge for industry and academia. Anthropol. Now 10 , 48–55 (2018).

Bowleg, L., Boone, C. A., Holt, S. L., del Río-González, A. M. & Mbaba, M. Beyond “heartfelt condolences”: a critical take on mainstream psychology’s responses to anti-Black police brutality. Am. Psychol. 77 , 362–380 (2022).

Chater, N. & Loewenstein, G. F. The i-frame and the s-frame: how focusing on the individual-level solutions has led behavioral public policy astray. Behav. Brain Sci . 5 , 1–60 (2022).

Onyeador, I. N., Hudson, S. T. J. & Lewis, N. A. Moving beyond implicit bias training: policy insights for increasing organizational diversity. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 8 , 19–26 (2021).

Lai, C. K. & Lisnek, J. A. The impact of implicit-bias-oriented diversity training on police officers’ beliefs, motivations, and actions. Psychol. Sci. 34 , 424–434 (2023).

Ray, V. A theory of racialized organizations. Am. Sociol. Rev. 84 , 26–53 (2019).

Bronfenbrenner, U. Ecology of the family as a context for human development: research perspectives. Dev. Psychol. 22 , 723–742 (1986).

Brady, L. M., Fryberg, S. A. & Shoda, Y. Expanding the interpretive power of psychological science by attending to culture. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 115 , 11406–11413 (2018).

Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. Cultures and selves: a cycle of mutual constitution. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5 , 420–430 (2010).

Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L., Sarmal, A., André, C. J. & Rogbeer, K. How microaggressions reinforce and perpetuate systemic racism in the U.S. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16 , 903–925 (2021). This paper lays out the ways in which individual-level biases (communicated through microaggressions) contribute to the perpetuation of systemic racism in the USA.

Dunbar-Ortiz, R. An Indigenous Peoples’ History Of The United States (Beacon, 2014).

Kendi, I. X. Stamped From The Beginning: The Definitive History Of Racist Ideas In America (Avalon, 2016). This book reviews the historical construction of race in the USA and how it has been used to manipulate individuals and systems.

Hannah-Jones, N. The 1619 Project: A New Origin Story (Penguin/Random House, 2021).

Kunnan, A. J. Politics and legislation in citizenship testing in the United States. Annu. Rev. Appl. Ling. 29 , 37–48 (2009).

Gamoran, A. The future of educational inequality: what went wrong and how can we fix it? Grant Foundation https://wtgrantfoundation.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/11/The-Future-of-Educational-Inequality-Adam-Gamoran.pdf (2015).

Hunt, B. & Whitman, S. Black:white health disparities in the United States and Chicago: 1990–2010. J. Racial Ethn. Health Dispar. 2 , 93–100 (2015).

Shapiro, T., Meschede, T. & Osoro, S. The Roots Of The Widening Racial Wealth Gap: Explaining The Black–White Economic Divide (Policy Brief) https://heller.brandeis.edu/iere/pdfs/racial-wealth-equity/racial-wealth-gap/roots-widening-racial-wealth-gap.pdf (Institute on Assets and Social Policy, 2013).

Wilkerson, I. Caste: The Origins Of Our Discontents (Random House, 2020).

Spiegelman, M. Race and ethnicity of public school teachers and their students. National Centre for Educational Statistics https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2020/2020103/index.asp (2020).

Melillo, G. 118th Congress is most racially diverse to date: research. The Hill https://thehill.com/changing-america/respect/diversity-inclusion/3805580-118th-congress-is-most-racially-diverse-to-date-research/ (2023).

ABA profile of the legal profession 2022 . ABA https://www.americanbar.org/content/dam/aba/administrative/news/2022/07/profile-report-2022.pdf (2022).

Strickland, B. Diversity among CEOs, CFOs continues to rise. J. Accountancy https://www.journalofaccountancy.com/news/2022/aug/diversity-among-ceos-cfos-continues-rise.html (2022).

Roberts, S. O. & Rizzo, M. T. The psychology of American racism. Am. Psychol. 76 , 475–487 (2021). This paper reviews contemporary factors that perpetuate racism in the USA.

García-Sánchez, E., Van der Toorn, J., Rodríguez-Bailón, R. & Willis, G. B. The vicious cycle of economic inequality: the role of ideology in shaping the relationship between “what is” and “what ought to be” in 41 countries. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 10 , 991–1001 (2019).

Kay, A. C. et al. Inequality, discrimination, and the power of the status quo: direct evidence for a motivation to see the way things are as the way they should be. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97 , 421–434 (2009).

Roberts, S. O. Descriptive-to-prescriptive (D2P) reasoning: an early emerging bias to maintain the status quo. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 33 , 289–322 (2022).

Roberts, S. O., Guo, C., Ho, A. K. & Gelman, S. A. Children’s descriptive-to-prescriptive tendency replicates (and varies) cross-culturally: evidence from China. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 165 , 148–160 (2018).

Roberts, S. O., Gelman, S. A. & Ho, A. K. So it is, so it shall be: group regularities license children’s prescriptive judgments. Cogn. Sci. 41 , 576–600 (2017).

Roberts, S. O., Ho, A. K. & Gelman, S. A. Group presence, category labels, and generic statements influence children to treat descriptive group regularities as prescriptive. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 158 , 19–31 (2017).

Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R. & Nosek, B. A. A decade of system justification theory: accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychol. 25 , 881–919 (2004).

Jost, J. T., Fitzsimons, G. & Kay, A. C. in Handbook Of Experimental Existential Psychology (eds Greenberg, J., Koole, S. L. & Pyszczynski, T.) 263–283 (Guilford, 2004).

Olson, K. R., Banaji, M. R., Dweck, C. S. & Spelke, E. S. Children’s biased evaluations of lucky versus unlucky people and their social groups. Psychol. Sci. 17 , 845–846 (2006).

Olson, K. R., Dweck, C. S., Spelke, E. S. & Banaji, M. R. Children’s responses to group-based inequalities: perpetuation and rectification. Soc. Cogn. 29 , 270–287 (2011).

Perry, S. P., Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L., Wages, J. E. & Abaied, J. L. Systemic considerations in child development and the pursuit of racial equality in the United States. Psychol. Inq. 32 , 180–186 (2021). This paper examines the topic of racial bias development from a systems perspective.

Vuletich, H. A. & Payne, B. K. Stability and change in implicit bias. Psychol. Sci. 30 , 854–886 (2019).

Craig, M. A., Rucker, J. M. & Richeson, J. A. Racial and political dynamics of an approaching “majority–minority” United States. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 677 , 204–214 (2018).

Craig, M. A., Rucker, J. M. & Richeson, J. A. The pitfalls and promise of increasing racial diversity: threat, contact, and race relations in the 21st century. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27 , 188–193 (2018).

Craig, M. A. & Richeson, J. A. More diverse yet less tolerant? How the increasingly diverse racial landscape affects white Americans’ racial attitudes. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40 , 750–761 (2014).

Skinner, A. L. & Cheadle, J. E. The “Obama effect”? Priming contemporary racial milestones increases implicit racial bias among whites. Soc. Cogn. 34 , 544–558 (2016).

Glaser, J., Spencer, K. & Charbonneau, A. Racial bias and public policy. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 1 , 88–94 (2014).

Piston, S. How explicit racial prejudice hurt Obama in the 2008 election. Polit. Behav. 32 , 431–451 (2010).

Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E. & Snowden, L. R. States higher in racial bias spend less on disabled Medicaid enrollees. Soc. Sci. Med. 208 , 150–157 (2018).

Harell, A. & Lieberman, E. How information about race-based health disparities affects policy preferences: evidence from a survey experiment about the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. Soc. Sci. Med. 277 , 113884 (2021).

Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L. et al. Highlighting COVID-19 racial disparities can reduce support for safety precautions among white U.S. residents. Soc. Sci. Med. 301 , 114951 (2022).

Stephens-Dougan, L. White Americans’ reactions to racial disparities in COVID-19. Am. Pol. Sci. Rev . 117 , 773–780 (2022).

Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E., Ayduk, O. & Mendoza-Denton, R. Blacks’ death rate due to circulatory diseases is positively related to whites’ explicit racial bias: a nationwide investigation using Project Implicit. Psychol. Sci. 27 , 1299–1311 (2016).

Riddle, T. & Sinclair, S. Racial disparities in school-based disciplinary actions are associated with county-level rates of racial bias. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 116 , 8255–8260 (2019).

Jones, J. M. Psychological knowledge and the new American dilemma of race. J. Soc. Issues 54 , 641–662 (1998).

Foster, S. J. The struggle for American identity: treatment of ethnic groups in United States history textbooks. Hist. Educ. 28 , 251–278 (1999).

Shuster K. Teaching hard history. Southern Poverty Law Center https://www.splcenter.org/20180131/teaching-hard-history (2018).

Tajima-Penña, R. Asian Americans [documentary film] (United States Public Broadcasting Services, 2020).

An, S. Asian Americans in American history: an AsianCrit perspective on Asian American inclusion in state U.S. history curriculum standards. Theory Res. Soc. Educ. 44 , 244–276 (2016).

Constante, A. How advocates are writing Asian American stories back into history books. NBC News https://www.nbcnews.com (2019).

Harada, V. H. The treatment of Asian Americans in U.S. history textbooks published 1994–1996. ERIC https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED448072.pdf (2000).

Lee, E. Why are Asian Americans missing from our textbooks? Pacific Standard https://psmag.com/news/why-are-asian-americans-missing-from-our-textbooks (2017).

Dai, J. D., Lopez, J. J., Brady, L. M., Eason, A. E. & Fryberg, S. A. Erasing and dehumanizing Natives to protect positive national identity: the Native mascot example. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 15 , 1–16 (2021).

Eason, A. E., Pope, T., Becenti, K. M. & Fryberg, S. A. Sanitizing history: national identification, negative stereotypes, and support for eliminating Columbus Day and adopting Indigenous Peoples Day. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 27 , 1–17 (2020).

Kurtiş, T., Adams, G. & Yellow Bird, M. Generosity or genocide? Identity implications of silence in American Thanksgiving commemorations. Memory 18 , 208–224 (2010).

Salter, P. S. & Adams, G. On the intentionality of cultural products: representations of Black history as psychological affordances. Front. Psychol. 7 , 1–21 (2016).

Sue, D. W. et al. Disarming racial microaggressions: microintervention strategies for targets, white allies, and bystanders. Am. Psychol. 74 , 128–142 (2019).

Smith, C. Unstated flag in Mississippi. Southeast. Geogr. 61 , 105–106 (2021).

Trenticosta, C. & Collins, W. C. Death and Dixie: how the courthouse Confederate flag influences capital cases in Louisiana. Harv. J. Racial Ethn. Just . 27 , 125–164 (2011).

O’Connell, H. A. Monuments outlive history: confederate monuments, the legacy of slavery, and black–white inequality. Ethn. Racial Stud. 43 , 460–478 (2020).

Morgan, H. Resisting the movement to ban critical race theory from schools. Clearing House J. Educ. Strateg. Issues Ideas 95 , 35–41 (2022).

Barrett, M. & Oppenheimer, L. Findings, theories and methods in the study of children’s national identifications and national attitudes. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 8 , 5–24 (2011).

Nelson, J. C., Adams, G. & Salter, P. S. The Marley hypothesis: denial of racism reflects ignorance of history. Psychol. Sci. 24 , 213–218 (2013).

Mukherjee, S., Salter, P. S. & Molina, L. E. Museum spaces as psychological affordances: representations of immigration history and national identity. Front. Psychol. 6 , 692 (2015).

Ehrlinger, J. et al. How exposure to the Confederate flag affects willingness to vote for Barack Obama. Political Psychol. 32 , 131–146 (2011).

Forbes, J. D. The historian and the Indian: racial bias in American history. Americas 19 , 349–362 (1963).

Smithers, G. D. Putting ethnohistory to work: Jack Forbes and the remaking of American historical consciousness. Ethnohistory 68 , 29–51 (2021).

Adams, G., Salter, P. S., Kurtiş, T., Naemi, P. & Estrada-Villalta, S. Subordinated knowledge as a tool for creative maladjustment and resistance to racial oppression. J. Soc. Issues 74 , 337–354 (2018).

Acuff, J. B. in The Palgrave Handbook Of Race And The Arts In Education (eds Kraehe, A., Gaztambide-Fernández, R. & Carpenter, B. II) 515–533 (Palgrave Macmillan, 2018).

Roy, W. G. & Dowd, T. J. What is sociological about music? Annu. Rev. Sociol. 36 , 183–203 (2010).

McCarthy-Brown, N. Decolonizing dance curriculum in higher education: one credit at a time. J. Dance Educ. 14 , 125–129 (2014).

Bradley, D. The sounds of silence: talking race in music education. Action Crit. Theory Music Educ. 6 , 132–162 (2007).

Kajikawa, L. in Seeing Race Again: Countering Colorblindness Across The Disciplines (eds Crenshaw, K. W., Harris, L. C, HoSang, D. M. & Lipsitz, G.) 155–174 (Univ. California Press, 2019).

Edwards, T. K. & Marshall, C. Undressing policy: a critical analysis of North Carolina (USA) public school dress codes. Gend. Educ. 32 , 732–750 (2020).

Greene, D. W. A multidimensional analysis of what not to wear in the workplace: hijabs and natural hair. FIU Law Rev. 8 , 333–362 (2013).

Armstrong, J. & Kuck, S. Saggy pants ordinance analysis: disparate impact versus respectability. Int. J. Bus. Soc. Sci . 11 , https://doi.org/10.30845/ijbss.v11n9p1 (2020).

Douglas, B., Lewis, C. W., Douglas, A., Scott, M. E. & Garrison-Wade, D. The impact of white teachers on the academic achievement of black students: an exploratory qualitative analysis. Educ. Found. 22 , 47–62 (2008).

Opie, T. R. & Phillips, K. W. Hair penalties: the negative influence of Afrocentric hair on ratings of Black women’s dominance and professionalism. Front. Psychol. 6 , 1311 (2015).

Koval, C. Z. & Rosette, A. S. The natural hair bias in job recruitment. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 12 , 741–750 (2020).

Inoue, A. B. Classroom writing assessment as an antiracist practice. Pedagogy 19 , 373–404 (2019).

Baugh, J. Beyond Ebonics: Linguistic Pride And Racial Prejudice (Oxford Univ. Press, 2000).

Craft, J. T., Wright, K. E., Weissler, R. E. & Queen, R. Language and discrimination: generating meaning, perceiving identities, and discriminating outcomes. Annu. Rev. Ling. 6 , 389–407 (2020).

Rickford, J. R. & King, S. Language and linguistics on trial: hearing Rachel Jeantel (and other vernacular speakers) in the courtroom and beyond. Language 92 , 948–988 (2016).

Williams, R. L. The Ebonics controversy. J. Black Psychol. 23 , 208–214 (1997).

Arya, R. Cultural appropriation: what it is and why it matters? Sociol. Compass 15 , 12923 (2021).

Chetty, R., Hendren, N., Jones, M. R. & Porter, S. R. Race and economic opportunity in the United States: an intergenerational perspective. Quar. J. Econ 135 , 711–783 (2020).

Piccard, A. Death by boarding school: the last acceptable racism and the United States’ genocide of Native Americans. Gonz. L. Rev. 49 , 137–185 (2013).

Nagle, R. This Land [podcast] https://crooked.com/podcast-series/this-land/ (Crooked Media, 2021).

Bishop, A. J. Western mathematics: the secret weapon of cultural imperialism. Race Class 32 , 51–65 (1990).

Iseke-Barnes, J. M. Ethnomathematics and language in decolonizing mathematics. Gend. Class 7 , 133–149 (2000).

Roberts, S. O., Bareket-Shavit, C., Dollins, F. A., Goldie, P. D. & Mortenson, E. Racial inequality in psychological research: trends of the past and recommendations for the future. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15 , 1295–1309 (2020).

Inoue, A. B. Antiracist Writing Assessment Ecologies: Teaching And Assessing Writing For A Socially Just Future (Parlor, 2015).

Eells, W. C. Number systems of the North American Indians. Am. Math. Mon. 20 , 263–272 (1913).

Mukherjee, S. & Perez, M. J. All Americans are not perceived as “true” Americans: implications for policy. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 8 , 34–41 (2021). This paper reviews evidence that American identity is associated with whiteness and highlights how this contributes to the perpetuation of racist systems.

Boykin, A. W. Experimental psychology from a black perspective: issues and examples. J. Black Psychol. 3 , 29–49 (1977).

Sue, D. W. Microaggressions and “evidence”: empirical or experiential reality? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12 , 170–172 (2017).

Johnston, P. M. G. Towards culturally appropriate assessment? A contribution to the debates. High. Educ. Q. 64 , 231–245 (2010).

Ledgerwood, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Jost, J. T. & Pohl, M. J. Working for the system: motivated defense of meritocratic beliefs. Soc. Cogn. 29 , 322–340 (2011).

McCoy, S. K. & Major, B. Priming meritocracy and the psychological justification of inequality. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 43 , 341–351 (2007).

Knowles, E. D. & Lowery, B. S. Meritocracy, self-concerns, and whites’ denial of racial inequity. Self Ident. 11 , 202–222 (2012).

Levy, S. R., West, T. L., Ramirez, L. & Karafantis, D. M. The protestant work ethic: a lay theory with dual intergroup implications. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 9 , 95–115 (2006).

Horowitz, J. M., Parker, K., Brown, A. & Cox, K. Amid national reckoning, Americans divided on whether increased focus on race will lead to major policy change. Pew Research Centre https://www.pewresearch.org/ (2020).

Wetts, R. & Willer, R. Who is called by the dog whistle? Experimental evidence that racial resentment and political ideology condition responses to racially encoded messages. Socius 5 , 2378023119866268 (2019).

Valentino, N. A., Hutchings, V. L. & White, I. K. Cues that matter: how political ads prime racial attitudes during campaigns. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 96 , 75–90 (2002).

Mendelberg, T. The Race Card (Princeton Univ. Press, 2017).

Saul, J. in New Work On Speech Acts (eds Fogal, D., Harris, D. W. & Moss, M.) 360–383 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2018).

Plaut, V. C., Thomas, K. M., Hurd, K. & Romano, C. A. Do color blindness and multiculturalism remedy or foster discrimination and racism? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27 , 200–206 (2018).

Apfelbaum, E. P., Pauker, K., Sommers, S. R. & Ambady, N. In blind pursuit of racial equality? Psychol. Sci. 21 , 1587–1592 (2010).

Federico, C. M. & Sidanius, J. Racism, ideology, and affirmative action, revisited: The antecedents and consequences of “principled objections” to affirmative action. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82 , 488–502 (2002).

Sidanius, J., Devereux, E. & Pratto, F. A comparison of symbolic racism theory and social dominance theory as explanations for racial policy attitudes. J. Soc. Psychol. 132 , 377–395 (1992).

Reynolds, J. & Xian, H. Perceptions of meritocracy in the land of opportunity. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 36 , 121–137 (2014).

Mazzocco, P. J., Cooper, L. W. & Flint, M. Different shades of racial colorblindness: the role of prejudice. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 15 , 167–178 (2012).

Whitley, B. E. Jr & Webster, G. D. The relationships of intergroup ideologies to ethnic prejudice: a meta-analysis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 23 , 207–237 (2019).

Neville, H. A., Lilly, R. L., Lee, R. M., Duran, G. & Browne, L. V. Construction and initial validation of the Color-Blind Racial Attitudes Scale (CoBRAS). J. Couns. Psychol. 47 , 59–70 (2000).

Rothstein, R. The Color Of Law: A Forgotten History Of How Our Government Segregated America (Liveright, 2017).

Mitchell, B. & Franco, J. HOLC “redlining” maps: the persistent structure of segregation and economic inequality. National Community Reinvestment Coalition https://ncrc.org/holc/ (2018).

Wilson, E. K. The legal foundations of white supremacy. DePaul J. Soc. Just. 11 , https://via.library.depaul.edu/jsj/vol11/iss2/6 (2018).

Anderson, D. Redlining’s legacy of inequality: $212,000 less home equity, low homeownership rates for Black families. Redfin News https://www.redfin.com/news/redlining-real-estate-racial-wealth-gap/ (2020).

Logan, J. R. & Stults, B. The Persistence of Segregation in the Metropolis: New Findings from the 2010 Census https://s4.ad.brown.edu/Projects/Diversity/data/report/report2.pdf (2011).

Ferguson, J. P. & Koning, R. Firm turnover and the return of racial establishment segregation. Am. Sociol. Rev. 83 , 445–474 (2018).

Brown v. Board of Education of Topeka, 347 U.S. 483 Justia https://supreme.justia.com/cases/federal/us/347/483/ (1954).

Office of Public Affairs. Court approves desegregation plan for Cleveland, Mississippi, schools: Cleveland School District to open consolidated middle and high schools by August 2017. US Department of Justice https://www.justice.gov/opa/pr/court-approves-desegregation-plan-cleveland-mississippi-schools (2017).

Francis, D. V. & Darity, W. A. Separate and unequal under one roof: how the legacy of racialized tracking perpetuates within-school segregation. RSF Russell Sage Found. J. Soc. Sci. 7 , 187–202 (2021).

Allport, G. W. The Nature of Prejudice (Addison Wesley, 1954).

Skinner, A. L. & Meltzoff, A. N. Childhood experiences and intergroup biases among children. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 13 , 211–240 (2019). This systematic review identifies the experiences that are empirically associated with intergroup biases in childhood.

Paluck, E. L., Green, S. A. & Green, D. P. The contact hypothesis re-evaluated. Behav. Public. Policy 3 , 129–158 (2019).

Pettigrew, T. F. & Tropp, L. R. A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90 , 751–783 (2006).

Pettigrew, T. F., Tropp, L. R., Wagner, U. & Christ, O. Recent advances in intergroup contact theory. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 35 , 271–280 (2011).

Rae, J. R., Newheiser, A.-K. & Olson, K. R. Exposure to racial out-groups and implicit race bias in the United States. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 6 , 535–543 (2015).

Rae, J. R., Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L., Reiman, A., Schmid, K. & Hewstone, M. Mixed evidence for interactive effects of outgroup proportions and intergroup contact on racial bias in the United States. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 13 , 476–489 (2022).

Quillian, L. Group threat and regional change in attitudes toward African-Americans. Am. J. Sociol. 102 , 816–860 (1996).

Bonam, C. M., Bergsieker, H. B. & Eberhardt, J. L. Polluting black space. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145 , 1561–1582 (2016).

Quillian, L. & Pager, D. Black neighbors, higher crime? The role of racial stereotypes in evaluations of neighborhood crime. Am. J. Sociol. 10 , 717–767 (2001).

Bonam, C. M., Taylor, V. J. & Yantis, C. Racialized physical space as cultural product. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 11 , 1–12 (2017).

Bonam, C., Yantis, C. & Taylor, V. J. Invisible middle-class Black space: asymmetrical person and space stereotyping at the race–class nexus. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 23 , 24–47 (2020).

Boykin, C. M. S. Negatively Stereotyping Historically Black Colleges and Universities as an Intergroup Process (Univ. California, 2018).

Yantis, C. & Bonam, C. M. Inconceivable middle-class black space: the architecture and consequences of space-focused stereotype content at the race–class nexus. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 47 , 1101–1118 (2021).

Howell, J. & Korver-Glenn, E. Neighborhoods, race, and the twenty-first-century housing appraisal industry. Sociol. Race Ethn. 4 , 473–490 (2018).

Sosina, V. E. & Weathers, E. S. Pathways to inequality: between-district segregation and racial disparities in school district expenditures. AERA Open 5 , https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858419872445 (2019).

Adams, C. Georgia destroyed a Black neighborhood. Now former residents want justice. NBC News https://www.nbcnews.com/news (2021).

Carpenter, D. M. & Ross, J. K. Testing O’Connor and Thomas: does the use of eminent domain target poor and minority communities? Urban Stud . 46 , 2447–2461 (2009).

Phillips, S. & Sillah, M. R. A house is not a home: effect of eminent domain abuse on the poor, African Americans, and the elderly. Hous. Soc. 36 , 115–136 (2009).

Fraley, J. M. Eminent domain and unfettered discretion: lessons from a history of US territorial takings. Penn State Law Rev. 126 , 609 (2021).

Banzhaf, S., Ma, L. & Timmins, C. Environmental justice: the economics of race, place, and pollution. J. Econ. Perspect. 33 , 185–208 (2019).

Blodgett, A. D. An analysis of pollution and community advocacy in ‘Cancer Alley’: setting an example for the environmental justice movement in St James Parish, Louisiana. Local Env. 11 , 647–661 (2006).

Anicich, E. M., Jachimowicz, J. M., Osborne, M. R. & Phillips, L. T. Structuring local environments to avoid racial diversity: anxiety drives whites’ geographical and institutional self-segregation preferences. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 95 , 104117 (2021).

DeLuca, J. R. Submersed in social segregation: the (re)production of social capital through swim club membership. J. Sport. Soc. Issues 37 , 340–363 (2013).

Gilchrist, J. & Parker, E. M. Racial and ethnic disparities in fatal unintentional drowning among persons less than 30 years of age — United States, 1999–2010. J. Saf. Res. 50 , 139–142 (2014).

Saluja, G. et al. Swimming pool drownings among US residents aged 5–24 years: understanding racial/ethnic disparities. Am. J. Public Health 96 , 728–733 (2006).

Wiltse, J. The Black–white swimming disparity in America: a deadly legacy of swimming pool discrimination. J. Sport. Soc. Issues 38 , 366–389 (2014).

Han, C. Geisha of a different kind: gay Asian men and the gendering of sexual identity. Sex. Cult. 10 , 3–28 (2006).

Sun, C., Liberman, R., Butler, A., Lee, S. Y. & Webb, R. Shifting receptions: Asian American stereotypes and the exploration of comprehensive media literacy. Commun. Rev. 18 , 294–314 (2015).

Chen, C. H. Feminization of Asian (American) men in the U.S. mass media: an analysis of The Ballad of Little Jo. J. Commun. Inq. 20 , 57–71 (1996).

Franklin, J. H. “Birth of a nation”: propaganda as history. Mass. Rev. 20 , 417–434 (1979).

Taylor, C. M. W. E. B. DuBois’s challenge to scientific racism. J. Black Stud. 11 , 449–460 (1981).

Melson-Silimon, A., Spivey, B. N. & Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L. The construction of racial stereotypes and how they serve as racial propaganda. Preprint at PsyArxiv https://psyarxiv.com/a2m84 (2023).

Dixon, T. L. & Linz, D. G. Overrepresentation and underrepresentation of African Americans and Latinos as lawbreakers on television news. J. Commun. 50 , 131–154 (2000).

Mastro, D. & Behm-Morawitz, E. Latino representation on primetime television. J. Mass. Commun. Q. 82 , 110–130 (2005).

Monk-Turner, E., Heiserman, M., Johnson, C., Cotton, V. & Jackson, M. The portrayal of racial minorities on prime time television: a replication of the Mastro and Greenberg study a decade later. Stud. Pop. Cul. 32 , 101–114 (2010).

Leavitt, P. A., Covarrubias, R., Perez, Y. A. & Fryberg, S. A. “Frozen in time”: the impact of Native American media representations on identity and self‐understanding. J. Soc. Issues 71 , 39–53 (2015).

Tukachinsky, R., Mastro, D. & Yarchi, M. Documenting portrayals of race/ethnicity on primetime television over a 20-year span and their association with national-level racial/ethnic attitudes. J. Soc. Issues 71 , 17–38 (2015).

Bjornstrom, E. E. S., Kaufman, R. L., Peterson, R. D. & Slater, M. D. Race and ethnic representations of lawbreakers and victims in crime news: a national study of television coverage. Soc. Probl. 57 , 269–293 (2011).

Dukes, K. N. & Gaither, S. E. Black racial stereotypes and victim blaming: implications for media coverage and criminal proceedings in cases of police violence against racial and ethnic minorities. J. Soc. Issues 73 , 789–807 (2017).

Sommers, S. R., Apfelbaum, E. P., Dukes, K. N., Toosi, N. & Wang, E. J. Race and media coverage of hurricane Katrina: analysis, implications, and future research questions. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 6 , 39–55 (2006).

Skinner, A. L., Perry, S. P. & Gaither, S. Not quite monoracial: biracial stereotypes explored. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 46 , 377–392 (2019).

Wages, J. E. III, Perry, S. P., Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L. & Bodenhausen, G. V. Reckless gambles and responsible ventures: racialized prototypes of risk-taking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol . 122 , 202–221 (2021).

2020 NCIC Missing person and unidentified person statistics. FBI https://www.fbi.gov/file-repository/2020-ncic-missing-person-and-unidentified-person-statistics.pdf/view (2021).

Sommers, Z. Missing white woman syndrome: an empirical analysis of race and gender disparities in online news coverage of missing persons. J. Crim. Law Criminol. 106 , 275–314 (2016).

Martin, D. et al. The spontaneous formation of stereotypes via cumulative cultural evolution. Psychol. Sci. 25 , 1777–1786 (2014).

Martin, D., Cunningham, S. J., Hutchison, J., Slessor, G. & Smith, K. How societal stereotypes might form and evolve via cumulative cultural evolution. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 11 , e12338 (2017).

Beukeboom, C. J. & Burgers, C. How stereotypes are shared through language: a review and introduction of the Social Categories and Stereotypes Communication (SCSC) framework. Rev. Commun. Res. 7 , 1–37 (2019).

Beukeboom, C. J. et al. The negation bias in stereotype maintenance: a replication in five languages. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 39 , 219–236 (2019).

Burgers, C. & Beukeboom, C. J. Stereotype transmission and maintenance through interpersonal communication: the irony bias. Commun. Res. 43 , 414–441 (2014).

Gorham, B. W. News media’s relationship with stereotyping: the linguistic intergroup bias in response to crime news. J. Commun. 56 , 289–308 (2006).

Kilgo, D. Media bias delegitimizes Black-rights protesters. Nature 593 , 315 (2021).

Greenwald, A. G. & Banaji, M. R. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self‐esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 102 , 4–27 (1995).

Macrae, C. N. & Bodenhausen, G. V. Social cognition: thinking categorically about others. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 51 , 93–120 (2000).

Busselle, R. & Crandall, H. Television viewing and perceptions about race differences in socioeconomic success. J. Broadcast. Electron. Media 46 , 265–282 (2002).

Oliver, M. B. & Armstrong, G. B. in Entertaining Crime: Television Reality Programs (eds Fishman, M. & Cavender, G.) 19–35 (Aldine de Gruyter, 1998).

Arendt, F. Cultivation effects of a newspaper on reality estimates and explicit and implicit attitudes. J. Media Psychol. 22 , 147–159 (2010).

Arendt, F. & Northup, T. Effects of long-term exposure to news stereotypes on implicit and explicit attitudes. Int. J. Commun. 9 , 732–751 (2015).

Lynott, D. et al. Are you what you read? Predicting implicit attitudes to immigration based on linguistic distributional cues from newspaper readership; a pre-registered study. Front. Psychol. 10 , 842 (2019).

Lai, C. & Banaji, M. in Difference Without Domination (eds Allen, D. & Somanathan, R.) 115–146 (Univ. Chicago Press, 2020).

Eberhardt, J. L., Goff, P. A., Purdie, V. J. & Davies, P. G. Seeing Black: race, crime, and visual processing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87 , 876–893 (2004).

Mekawi, Y. & Bresin, K. Is the evidence from racial bias shooting task studies a smoking gun? Results from a meta-analysis. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 61 , 120–130 (2015).

Cooley, E. et al. Racial biases in officers’ decisions to frisk are amplified for Black people stopped among groups leading to similar biases in searches, arrests, and use of force. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 11 , 761–769 (2020).

Goel, S., Rao, J. M. & Shroff, R. Precinct or prejudice? Understanding racial disparities in New York City’s stop-and-frisk policy. Ann. Appl. Stat. 10 , 365–394 (2016).

Hetey, R. C., Monin, B., Maitreyi, A. & Eberhardt, J. L. Data for change: a statistical analysis of police stops, searches, handcuffings, and arrests in Oakland, Calif., 2013–2014. Stanford SPARQ https://searchworks.stanford.edu/view/by412gh2838 (2016).

Levchak, P. J. Stop-and-frisk in New York City: estimating racial disparities in post-stop outcomes. J. Crim. Justice 73 , 101784 (2021).

Filindra, A. & Kaplan, N. J. Racial resentment and whites’ gun policy preferences in contemporary America. Pol. Behav. 38 , 255–275 (2016).

O’Brien, K., Forrest, W., Lynott, D. & Daly, M. Racism, gun ownership and gun control: biased attitudes in US whites may influence policy decisions. PLoS One 8 , e77552 (2013).

Higgenbotham, G. D., Sears, D. O. & Goldstein, L. When an irresistible prejudice meets immovable politics: Black legal gun ownership undermines racially resentful white Americans’ gun rights advocacy. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 152 , 410–424 (2023).

Hayes, M., Fortunato, D. & Hibbing, M. V. Race–gender bias in white Americans’ preferences for gun availability. J. Public Policy 41 , 818–834 (2021).

Walker, H., Collingwood, L. & Bunyasi, T. L. White response to black death: a racialized theory of white attitudes towards gun control. Du Bois Rev. 17 , 165–188 (2020).

Hehman, E., Flake, J. K. & Calanchini, J. Disproportionate use of lethal force in policing is associated with regional racial biases of residents. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 9 , 393–401 (2017).

Alexander, M. The New Jim Crow: Mass Incarceration In The Age Of Colorblindness (New Press, 2010). This book examines how policing policies and widespread racial biases work together to marginalize and control people of colour in the USA.

Carson, E. A. Prisoners in 2019. Bureau of Justice Statistics https://bjs.ojp.gov/content/pub/pdf/p19.pdf (2020).

Skinner, A. L. & Haas, I. J. Perceived threat associated with police officers and Black men predicts support for policing policy reform. Front. Psychol. 7 , 1057 (2016).

Hetey, R. C. & Eberhardt, J. L. The numbers don’t speak for themselves: racial disparities and the persistence of inequality in the criminal justice system. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27 , 183–187 (2018).

Peffley, M. & Hurwitz, J. Persuasion and resistance: race and the death penalty in America. Am. J. Pol. Sci. 51 , 996–1012 (2007).

Hetey, R. C. & Eberhardt, J. L. Racial disparities in incarceration increase acceptance of punitive policies. Psychol. Sci. 25 , 1949–1954 (2014).

Brey, E. & Shutts, K. Children use nonverbal cues from an adult to evaluate peers. J. Cogn. Dev. 19 , 121–136 (2018).

Weisbuch, M., Pauker, K. & Ambady, N. The subtle transmission of race bias via televised nonverbal behavior. Science 326 , 1711–1714 (2009).

Ritterhouse, J. L. Growing Up Jim Crow: How Black And White Southern Children Learned Race (Univ. North Carolina Press, 2006).

Camp, N. P., Voigt, R., Jurafsky, D. & Eberhardt, J. L. The thin blue waveform: racial disparities in officer prosody undermine institutional trust in the police. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 121 , 1157–1171 (2021).

Voigt, R. et al. Language from police body camera footage shows racial disparities in officer respect. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 114 , 6521–6526 (2017).

Skinner, A. L., Meltzoff, A. N. & Olson, K. R. “Catching” social bias: exposure to biased nonverbal signals creates social biases in preschool children. Psychol. Sci. 28 , 216–224 (2017).

Skinner, A. L., Olson, K. R. & Meltzoff, A. N. Acquiring group bias: observing other people’s nonverbal signals can create social group biases. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 119 , 824–838 (2020).

Skinner, A. L. & Perry, S. Are attitudes contagious? Exposure to biased nonverbal signals can create novel social attitudes. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 46 , 514–524 (2020).

Castelli, L., Carraro, L., Pavan, G., Murelli, E. & Carraro, A. The power of the unsaid: the influence of nonverbal cues on implicit attitudes. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol 42 , 1376–1393 (2012).

Castelli, L., De Dea, C. & Nesdale, D. Learning social attitudes: children’s sensitivity to the nonverbal behaviors of adult models during interracial interactions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34 , 1504–1513 (2008).

Willard, G., Isaac, K.-J. & Carney, D. R. Some evidence for the nonverbal contagion of racial bias. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 128 , 96–107 (2015).

Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L. & Sánchez, S. The nonverbal spread of group prejudice and preliminary evidence of strategies to mitigate prejudice transmission. Preprint at OSFpreprints https://osf.io/8cyg2 (2022).

Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B. & Howard, A. On the nature of prejudice: automatic and controlled processes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 33 , 510–540 (1997).

Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K. & Gaertner, S. L. Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82 , 62–68 (2002).

Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C. & Williams, C. J. Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69 , 1013–1027 (1995).

McConnell, A. R. & Liebold, J. M. Relations among the implicit association test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37 , 435–442 (2001).

Skinner, A. L., Osnaya, A., Patel, B. & Perry, S. P. Mimicking others’ nonverbal signals is associated with increased attitude contagion. J. Nonverbal Behav. 44 , 117–131 (2020).

Weisbuch, M. & Pauker, K. The nonverbal transmission of intergroup bias: a model of bias contagion with implications for social policy. Soc. Issues Policy Rev 5 , 257–291 (2011).

Vittrup, B. Color blind or color conscious? White American mothers’ approaches to racial socialization. J. Fam. Issues 39 , 668–692 (2018).

Zucker, J. K. & Patterson, M. M. Racial socialization practices among white American parents: relations to racial attitudes, racial identity, and school diversity. J. Fam. Issues 39 , 3903–3930 (2018).

Scott, K. E., Shutts, K. & Devine, P. G. Parents’ role in addressing children’s racial bias: the case of speculation without evidence. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15 , 1178–1186 (2020).

Loyd, A. B. & Gaither, S. E. Racial/ethnic socialization for white youth: what we know and future directions. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 59 , 54–64 (2018).

Perry, S. P., Skinner-Dorkenoo, A. L., Abaied, J. L. & Waters, S. F. Applying the evidence we have: support for having race conversations in white U.S. families. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17 , 895–900 (2022). This paper reviews the empirical evidence on the impact of racial socialization on the racial biases of white children in the USA.

Perry, S. P., Skinner, A. L., Abaied, J., Osnaya, A. & Waters, S. F. Exploring how parent–child conversations about race influence children’s implicit biases. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3xdg8 (2021).

Hagerman, M. White Kids: Growing Up With Privilege In A Racially Divided America (New York Univ. Press, 2018).

Hagerman, M. A. White racial socialization: progressive fathers on raising “antiracist” children. J. Marriage Fam. 79 , 60–74 (2017).

Katz, P. A. Racists or tolerant multiculturalists? How do they begin? Am. Psychol. 58 , 897–909 (2003).

Vittrup, B. & Holden, G. W. Exploring the impact of educational television and parent–child discussions on children’s racial attitudes. Anal. Soc. Issues Public. Policy 11 , 82–104 (2011).

Cameron, L. & Rutland, A. Extended contact through story reading in school: reducing children’s prejudice toward the disabled. J. Soc. Issues 62 , 469–488 (2006).

Cameron, L., Rutland, A., Brown, R. & Douch, R. Changing children’s intergroup attitudes toward refugees: testing different models of extended contact. Child. Dev. 77 , 1208–1219 (2006).

Cameron, L., Rutland, A. & Brown, R. Promoting children’s positive intergroup attitudes towards stigmatized groups: extended contact and multiple classification skills training. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 31 , 454–466 (2007).

Cameron, L., Rutland, A., Hossain, R. & Petley, R. When and why does extended contact work? The role of high quality direct contact and group norms in the development of positive ethnic intergroup attitudes amongst children. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 14 , 193–206 (2011).

Cameron, L., Rutland, A., Turner, R., Holman-Nicolas, R. & Powell, C. “Changing attitudes with a little imagination”: imagined contact effects on young children’s intergroup bias. An. Psicol. 27 , 708–717 (2011).

Turner, R. N., Hewstone, M. & Voci, A. Reducing explicit and implicit outgroup prejudice via direct and extended contact: the mediating role of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93 , 369–388 (2007).

Vezzali, L., Stathi, S., Giovannini, D., Capozza, D. & Visintin, E. P. “And the best essay is…”: extended contact and cross-group friendships at school. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 54 , 601–615 (2015).

Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Austin, A. J. & Cox, W. T. L. Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: a prejudice habit-breaking intervention. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48 , 1267–1278 (2012).

Parker, L. R., Monteith, M. J., Moss-Racusin, C. A. & Van Camp, A. R. Promoting concern about gender bias with evidence-based confrontation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 74 , 8–23 (2018).

Perry, S. P., Dovidio, J. F., Murphy, M. C. & van Ryn, M. The joint effect of bias awareness and self-reported prejudice on intergroup anxiety and intentions for intergroup contact. Cultur. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 21 , 89–96 (2015).

Haney-López, I. Dog Whistle Politics: How Coded Racial Appeals Have Reinvented Racism And Wrecked The Middle Class (Oxford Univ. Press, 2014).

Mendelberg, T. The Race Card: Campaign Strategy, Implicit Messages, And The Norm of Equality (Princeton Univ. Press, 2001).

Perry, S. P., Skinner, A. L. & Abaied, J. L. Bias awareness predicts color conscious racial socialization methods among white parents. J. Soc. Issues 75 , 1035–1056 (2019). This paper examines how bias awareness relates to parental racial socialization and acknowledgment of systemic racism.

Zell, E. & Lesick, T. L. Ignorance of history and political differences in perception of racism in the United States. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 13 , 1022–1031 (2022).

Bonam, C. M., Nair Das, V., Coleman, B. R. & Salter, P. Ignoring history, denying racism: mounting evidence for the Marley hypothesis and epistemologies of ignorance. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 10 , 257–265 (2019). This paper provides evidence of the impact of educating the public in the USA about historical racial injustices.

Hughes, J. M., Bigler, R. S. & Levy, S. R. Consequences of learning about historical racism among European American and African American children. Child. Dev. 78 , 1689–1705 (2007). This paper provides evidence of the impact of educating white children in the USA about historical racial injustices.

Neufeld, K. H. S., Starzyk, K. B., Boese, G. D., Efimoff, I. H. & Wright, S. “The more you know”: critical historical knowledge about Indian residential schools increases non-Indigenous Canadians’ empathy for Indigenous Peoples. Polit. Psychol. 43 , 617–633 (2022).

Adams, G., Dobles, I., Gómez, L. H., Kurtiş, T. & Molina, L. E. Decolonizing psychological science: introduction to the special thematic section. J. Soc. Polit. Psychol. 3 , 213–238 (2015).

Cheon, B. K., Melani, I. & Hong, Y. Y. How USA-centric is psychology? An archival study of implicit assumptions of generalizability of findings to human nature based on origins of study samples. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 11 , 928–937 (2020).

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33 , 61–83 (2010).

Anderson, C. White Rage: The Unspoken Truth Of Our Racial Divide (Bloomsbury, 2016). This book examines the history of racial progress in the USA, highlighting how the racial biases of white residents and biased systems have worked in concert to maintain racial oppression over the course of USA history.

Sommers, S. R. & Norton, M. I. Race-based judgments, race-neutral justifications: experimental examination of peremptory use and the Batson challenge procedure. Law Hum. Behav. 31 , 261–273 (2007).

Raphael, M. & Ungvarsky, E. Excuses, excuses: neutral explanations under Batson v. Kentucky . Univ. Mich. J. Law Reform. 27 , 229–275 (1993).

Rosino, M. L. & Hughey, M. W. The war on drugs, racial meanings, and structural racism: a holistic and reproductive approach. Am. J. Econ. Sociol. 77 , 849–892 (2018).

Wessler, M. Updated charts provide insights on racial disparities, correctional control, jail suicides, and more. Prison Policy Initiative https://www.prisonpolicy.org/blog/2022/05/19/updated_charts/ (2022).

Nellis, A. The color of justice: racial and ethnic disparities in state prisons. The Sentencing Project https://www.sentencingproject.org/reports/the-color-of-justice-racial-and-ethnic-disparity-in-state-prisons-the-sentencing-project/ (2021).

Dixon, J., Tredoux, C., Durrheim, K., Finchilescu, G. & Clack, B. ‘The inner citadels of the color line’: mapping the micro‐ecology of racial segregation in everyday life spaces. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2 , 1547–1569 (2008).

Durrheim, K., Quayle, M., Whitehead, K. & Kriel, A. Denying racism: discursive strategies used by the South African media. Crit. Arts 19 , 167–186 (2005).

Race in South Africa: ‘We haven’t learnt we are human beings first.’ BBC News https://www.bbc.com/news/world-africa-55333625 (2021).

Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., Katz, R. C., Tredoux, C. & Spelke, E. S. Race preferences in children: insights from South Africa. Dev. Sci . 14 , 1283–1291 (2011).

Dunham, Y., Srinivasan, M., Dotsch, R. & Barner, D. Religion insulates ingroup evaluations: the development of intergroup attitudes in India. Dev. Sci. 17 , 311–319 (2014).

Bar-Tal, D. Intractable Conflicts: Socio-Psychological Foundations And Dynamics (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2011).

Bar-Tal, D., Diamond, A. H. & Nasie, M. Political socialization of young children in intractable conflicts: conception and evidence. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 41 , 415–425 (2017).

Diesendruck, G., Birnbaum, D., Deeb, I., Segall, G. & Ben-Eliyahu, A. The development of social essentialism: the case of Israeli children’s inferences about Jews and Arabs. Child. Dev. 81 , 757–777 (2010).

Nasie, M., Diamond, A. H. & Bar-Tal, D. Young children in intractable conflicts: the Israeli case. Person. Soc. Psych. Rev. 20 , 365–392 (2016).

Teichman, Y. The development of Israeli children’s images of Jews and Arabs and their expression in human figure drawings. Dev. Psych. 37 , 749–761 (2001).

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank K. G. Rogbeer, J. Tran and B. MacDonald for assistance in proofreading and formatting earlier drafts of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA

Allison L. Skinner-Dorkenoo

Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Meghan George, Sirenia Sánchez & Sylvia P. Perry

Department of Psychology and Counseling, University of Central Arkansas, Conway, AR, USA

James E. Wages III

Institute for Policy Research, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Sylvia P. Perry

Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

A.L.S.-D. researched data for the article. All authors wrote the article. All authors reviewed and/or edited the manuscript before submission.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Allison L. Skinner-Dorkenoo or Sylvia P. Perry .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information.

Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Boaz Hameiri, Keith Payne and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Skinner-Dorkenoo, A.L., George, M., Wages, J.E. et al. A systemic approach to the psychology of racial bias within individuals and society. Nat Rev Psychol 2 , 392–406 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00190-z

Download citation

Accepted : 19 April 2023

Published : 18 May 2023

Issue Date : July 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00190-z

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

National institutes of health r-series grants portfolio of racism and healthcare, 2017–2022.

  • Judith Aponte
  • Maria Isabel Roldós

BMC Public Health (2023)

Going beyond the individual level in self-control research

  • Wilhelm Hofmann

Nature Reviews Psychology (2023)

The case against implicit bias fatalism

  • Benedek Kurdi
  • Eric Mandelbaum

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

essay on racial bias

Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education Essay

  • To find inspiration for your paper and overcome writer’s block
  • As a source of information (ensure proper referencing)
  • As a template for you assignment

The Impact of Racism in Schools and on the Mental Health of Students

Funding is one of the main factors that ensure racial segregation and exacerbation of the plight of the black population. Being initially in a more disastrous economic situation, racial minority populations fall into a vicious circle. Low-funded schools in poor areas have low academic ratings, which further contributes to the reduction of the material base. Due to their poor academic performance and the need to earn a living, many minorities are deprived of the opportunity to receive prestigious higher education. They are left with low-skilled jobs, which makes it impossible for their children to go to private school or move to a prestigious area with well-funded public schools. In institutions with little funding, unfortunately, manifestations of racism still prevail.

A significant factor in systemic racism in modern schools is the theory of colorblindness as the prevailing ideology in schools and pedagogical universities. The total avoidance of racial topics in schools has led to a complete absence of material related to the culture of racial minorities in the curricula. An example is the complaint of the parents of one of the black students that, during the passage of civilizations, the Greeks, Romans, and Incas were discussed in the lessons, but nothing was said about Africa. However, there were a few African American students in the class (Yi et al., 2022). The white director justified herself by saying that this was the curriculum and that it was not customary at school to divide people by skin color. In response, the student’s mother stated that children have eyes, and they see everything. And she would like them to see that we had a strong and fruitful culture. This state of affairs is justified by the proponents of assimilationism and American patriotism, built mainly around the honoring of the merits of white settlers and the founding fathers.

Meanwhile, the works of many researchers provide evidence that a high level of colorblindness among students correlates with greater racial intolerance. One study on race relations was conducted among young “millennials”. As a result, thousands of reports were recorded of openly racist statements and actions of white people from the field of view of these students (Plaut, et al., 2018). Another study on colorblindness found that white students who avoid mentioning racial issues were less friendly on assignments with black partners. This could be because they have less eye contact.

The shortcomings of the described situation affect not only black students but also white teachers who have not received proper training in their time on how best to take into account the characteristics of students from racial minorities. One researcher writes that in his entire experience in multicultural education, he faced the almost universal embarrassment that racial issues caused to white teachers. A common complaint is: I feel helpless. What am I, as a white teacher, to do? One educator remarked that he had never seen African-American teachers say that they did not distinguish between races (Mekawi et al., 2017). This is further proof that racism and the factors leading to it contribute only to the split of social ties at school. Students from racial minorities feel this burden the most, which leads to their feeling of constant alienation. During the school years, conflicts with children “not like the rest” are especially aggravated – the state of affairs described above provides the basis for constant skirmishes, fights, and tension in institutions.

Suggestions for Creating an Inclusive School Environment

Among the educational factors supporting the status quo of widespread structural racism are the following. This is the system of financing public schools and the dominance of the ideology of colorblindness in schools and pedagogical universities. In the opposite direction, there is such a factor as the peculiarity of keeping educational statistics (Welton, et al., 2018). By providing up-to-date information on the state of affairs of students of various racial and ethnic groups, statistics give rise to the search for optimal solutions in the field of school policy.

The inclusion of racial and ethnic dimensions in educational statistics is intended to provide an objective assessment of the current situation regarding racial differences in American society in order to develop and improve racially relevant policies. In recent years, the ideas of culturally relevant pedagogy have been actively promoted in the US educational sphere. American citizens are becoming more interested and enlightened in the field of racial issues, which can be seen in activist speeches and anti-racist public actions.

It is crucial to teach racism in schools so that all pupils may understand what it is, how it affects, and how to stop tolerating it. There are many publications and learning experience plans that address racism. It is essential to ask teachers and principals to integrate lessons on racism into the syllabus. One can also request that your teachers incorporate novels with a variety of subjects (Welton, et al., 2018). Then, it is important to request that the school draft an inclusion and zero-tolerance statement. Counselors can encourage the instructors and administration to implement these policies at the school if they do not already exist in the code of conduct or other policies (Pizarro & Kohli, 2020). It is critical that schools have clear policies about race and how individuals are treated on campus.

Resources for the School Counselor to Deal With Prejudice and Its Impact at the School

Mekawi, Y., Bresin, K. & Hunter, C.D. (2017). Who is more likely to “not see race”? individual differences in racial colorblindness. Race and Social Problems, 9 (1), 207–217. Web.

The authors claim that many Americans support a colorblind racial philosophy, which emphasizes sameness and the equitable allocation of resources without regard to race. The current study looked at the relationships between aggressiveness, and empathy in white undergraduates and three distinct types of racial colorblindness, including ignorance of racial privilege, ignorance of institutional discrimination, and ignorance of overt racism. The findings showed two distinct trends. In contrast to ignorance of overt racism and institutional discrimination, which were linked to poorer cooperativeness, cognitive flexibility, and empathic concern, ignorance of racial privilege was associated with lower openness and viewpoint-taking. These findings are addressed in light of a larger body of research on bias and personality.

Pizarro, M., & Kohli, R. (2020). “I stopped sleeping”: Teachers of color and the impact of racial Battle Fatigue. Urban Education, 55 (7), 967–991. Web.

According to the authors, an operational definition of racial battle fatigue (RBF) is the mental, emotional, and physical costs of fighting racism. RBF is employed in this article to examine the effects of racism on educators of color who work in a predominately “White profession.” The scholars share counterstories of urban academics of color who confront racism on a regular basis in their workplaces. This has a negative effect on their well-being and ability to stay in the profession. The authors also discuss their resiliency and resistance tactics since they depend on a supportive community to persevere and change their schools.

Plaut, V. C., Thomas, K. M., Hurd, K., & Romano, C. A. (2018). Do Color blindness and multiculturalism remedy or foster discrimination and racism? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27 (3), 200–206. Web.

This article gives psychology science’s perspective on the question of whether multiculturalism and colorblindness are more likely to prevent prejudice and racism than they are to promote it. The authors first concentrate on the results of a color-blind model. The study in this area reveals that while colorblindness may be appealing to certain people, it can also make people less sensitive to racism and prejudice. Additionally, according to the literature, color blindness generally has detrimental effects on intergroup relationships, minorities’ perceptions and results, and the promotion of diversity and inclusion in organizational settings. In the second section, the scholars look at the situations in which a multicultural perspective has beneficial or bad effects on intergroup relations, organizational diversity initiatives, and discrimination.

Welton, A. D., Owens, D. R., & Zamani-Gallaher, E. M. (2018). Anti-racist change: A conceptual framework for educational institutions to take systemic action. Teachers College Record, 120 (14), 1–22. Web.

In order to attain racial justice in education, people’s mindsets must also be changed to embrace a more anti-racist worldview. In order to investigate whether behaviors and leadership qualities could really encourage institutional change for racial justice, the authors review two sets of literature: studies on anti-racism and institutional transformation. However, they admit the constraints of each set of studies. The organizational transformation research often ignores equity concerns, notably racial conversations, while anti-racism research is more ideological and theoretical. The scholars combine essential ideas from the literature on organizational change and anti-racism to propose a conceptual framework that may be utilized to create a systematic anti-racist change at a wide level.

Yi, J., Neville, H. A., Todd, N. R., & Mekawi, Y. (2022). Ignoring race and denying racism: A meta-analysis of the associations between colorblind racial ideology, anti-Blackness, and other variables antithetical to racial justice . Journal of Counseling Psychology . Web.

The authors sought to comprehend how colorblind racial ideology (CBRI), or the rejection and minimizing of race and racism, can act as an obstacle to engaging in antiracist practice by relying on antiracism research. To find out if color evasion (ignorance of race) and power evasion (defiance of structural racism) CBRI were differently connected with anti-Blackness and mechanisms related to antiracism, the scholars specifically performed a meta-analysis. Results from 83 research with more than 25,000 participants and 375 effects reveal that varied effects depend on the kind of CBRI. The area of counseling psychology may be pushed by this meta-analysis to construct a bridge between different ideologies and the development of systemic reform.

  • Empathy and Racism in Stockett's The Help and Li's To Kill a Mockingbird
  • Causes, Facilitators, and Solutions to Racism
  • Colorblind Society: Race and Skin Color in America
  • Anti-Racism in Shakespeare’s Othello
  • The Bill H.R.666 Anti-Racism in Public Health Act of 2021
  • Vertical Lines of Power or Authority
  • Racism in the Healthcare Sector
  • Unraveling the Science of Human Races
  • Racism in Healthcare and Education
  • Portrayal of Races: Content Analysis and Literature Review
  • Chicago (A-D)
  • Chicago (N-B)

IvyPanda. (2023, November 25). Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education. https://ivypanda.com/essays/racial-discrimination-and-justice-in-education/

"Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education." IvyPanda , 25 Nov. 2023, ivypanda.com/essays/racial-discrimination-and-justice-in-education/.

IvyPanda . (2023) 'Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education'. 25 November.

IvyPanda . 2023. "Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education." November 25, 2023. https://ivypanda.com/essays/racial-discrimination-and-justice-in-education/.

1. IvyPanda . "Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education." November 25, 2023. https://ivypanda.com/essays/racial-discrimination-and-justice-in-education/.

Bibliography

IvyPanda . "Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education." November 25, 2023. https://ivypanda.com/essays/racial-discrimination-and-justice-in-education/.

  • Skip to main content
  • Keyboard shortcuts for audio player

Code Switch

  • School Colors
  • Perspectives

Code Switch

The Code Switch Podcast

  • LISTEN & FOLLOW
  • Apple Podcasts
  • Amazon Music

Your support helps make our show possible and unlocks access to our sponsor-free feed.

Hear Something, Say Something: Navigating The World Of Racial Awkwardness

Listen to this week's episode.

We've all been there — confronted with something shy of overt racism, but charged enough to make us uncomfortable. So what do you do?

We've all been there — having fun relaxing with friends and family, when someone says something a little racially off. Sometimes it's subtle, like the friend who calls Thai food "exotic." Other times it's more overt, like that in-law who's always going on about "the illegals."

In any case, it can be hard to know how to respond. Even the most level-headed among us have faltered trying to navigate the fraught world of racial awkwardness.

So what exactly do you do? We delve into the issue on this week's episode of the Code Switch podcast, featuring writer Nicole Chung and Code Switch's Shereen Marisol Meraji, Gene Demby and Karen Grigsby Bates.

We also asked some folks to write about what runs through their minds during these tense moments, and how they've responded (or not). Their reactions ran the gamut from righteous indignation to total passivity, but in the wake of these uncomfortable comments, everyone seemed to walk away wishing they'd done something else.

Aaron E. Sanchez

It was the first time my dad visited me at college, and he had just dropped me off at my dorm. My suitemate walked in and sneered.

"Was that your dad?" he asked. "He looks sooo Mexican."

essay on racial bias

Aaron E. Sanchez is a Texas-based writer who focuses on issues of race, politics and popular culture from a Latino perspective. Courtesy of Aaron Sanchez hide caption

He kept laughing about it as he left my room.

I was caught off-guard. Instantly, I grew self-conscious, not because I was ashamed of my father, but because my respectability politics ran deep. My appearance was supposed to be impeccable and my manners unimpeachable to protect against stereotypes and slights. I felt exposed.

To be sure, when my dad walked into restaurants and stores, people almost always spoke to him in Spanish. He didn't mind. The fluidity of his bilingualism rarely failed him. He was unassuming. He wore his working-class past on his frame and in his actions. He enjoyed hard work and appreciated it in others. Yet others mistook him for something altogether different.

People regularly confused his humility for servility. He was mistaken for a landscape worker, a janitor, and once he sat next to a gentleman on a plane who kept referring to him as a "wetback." He was a poor Mexican-American kid who grew up in the Segundo Barrio of El Paso, Texas, for certain. But he was also an Air Force veteran who had served for 20 years. He was an electrical engineer, a proud father, an admirable storyteller, and a pretty decent fisherman.

I didn't respond to my suitemate. To him, my father was a funny caricature, a curio he could pick up, purchase and discard. And as much as it was hidden beneath my elite, liberal arts education, I was a novelty to him too, an even rarer one at that. Instead of a serape, I came wrapped in the trappings of middle-classness, a costume I was trying desperately to wear convincingly.

That night, I realized that no clothing or ill-fitting costume could cover us. Our bodies were incongruous to our surroundings. No matter how comfortable we were in our skins, our presence would make others uncomfortable.

Karen Good Marable

When the Q train pulled into the Cortelyou Road station, it was dark and I was tired. Another nine hours in New York City, working in the madness that is Midtown as a fact-checker at a fashion magazine. All day long, I researched and confirmed information relating to beauty, fashion and celebrity, and, at least once a day, suffered an editor who was openly annoyed that I'd discovered an error. Then, the crush of the rush-hour subway, and a dinner obligation I had to fulfill before heading home to my cat.

essay on racial bias

Karen Good Marable is a writer living in New York City. Her work has been featured in publications like The Undefeated and The New Yorker. Courtesy of Karen Good Marable hide caption

The train doors opened and I turned the corner to walk up the stairs. Coming down were two girls — free, white and in their 20s . They were dancing as they descended, complete with necks rolling, mouths pursed — a poor affectation of black girls — and rapping as they passed me:

Now I ain't sayin she a golddigger/But she ain't messin' with no broke niggas!

That last part — broke niggas — was actually less rap, more squeals that dissolved into giggles. These white girls were thrilled to say the word publicly — joyously, even — with the permission of Kanye West.

I stopped, turned around and stared at them. I envisioned kicking them both squarely in their backs. God didn't give me telekinetic powers for just this reason. I willed them to turn around and face me, but they did not dare. They bopped on down the stairs and onto the platform, not evening knowing the rest of the rhyme.

Listen: I'm a black woman from the South. I was born in the '70s and raised by parents — both educators — who marched for their civil rights. I never could get used to nigga being bandied about — not by the black kids and certainly not by white folks. I blamed the girls' parents for not taking over where common sense had clearly failed. Hell, even radio didn't play the nigga part.

I especially blamed Kanye West for not only making the damn song, but for having the nerve to make nigga a part of the damn hook.

Life in NYC is full of moments like this, where something happens and you wonder if you should speak up or stay silent (which can also feel like complicity). I am the type who will speak up . Boys (or men) cussing incessantly in my presence? Girls on the train cussing around my 70-year-old mama? C'mon, y'all. Do you see me? Do you hear yourselves? Please. Stop.

But on this day, I just didn't feel like running down the stairs to tap those girls on the shoulder and school them on what they damn well already knew. On this day, I just sighed a great sigh, walked up the stairs, past the turnstiles and into the night.

Robyn Henderson-Espinoza

When I was 5 or 6, my mother asked me a question: "Does anyone ever make fun of you for the color of your skin?"

This surprised me. I was born to a Mexican woman who had married an Anglo man, and I was fairly light-skinned compared to the earth-brown hue of my mother. When she asked me that question, I began to understand that I was different.

essay on racial bias

Robyn Henderson-Espinoza is a visiting assistant professor of ethics at the Pacific School of Religion in Berkeley, Calif. Courtesy of Robyn Henderson-Espinoza hide caption

Following my parents' divorce in the early 1980s, I spent a considerable amount of time with my father and my paternal grandparents. One day in May of 1989, I was sitting at my grandparents' dinner table in West Texas. I was 12. The adults were talking about the need for more laborers on my grandfather's farm, and my dad said this:

"Mexicans are lazy."

He called the undocumented workers he employed on his 40 acres "wetbacks." Again and again, I heard from him that Mexicans always had to be told what to do. He and friends would say this when I was within earshot. I felt uncomfortable. Why would my father say these things about people like me?

But I remained silent.

It haunts me that I didn't speak up. Not then. Not ever. I still hear his words, 10 years since he passed away, and wonder whether he thought I was a lazy Mexican, too. I wish I could have found the courage to tell him that Mexicans are some of the hardest-working people I know; that those brown bodies who worked on his property made his lifestyle possible.

As I grew in experience and understanding, I was able to find language that described what he was doing: stereotyping, undermining, demonizing. I found my voice in the academy and in the movement for black and brown lives.

Still, the silence haunts me.

Channing Kennedy

My 20s were defined in no small part by a friendship with a guy I never met. For years, over email and chat, we shared everything with each other, and we made great jokes. Those jokes — made for each other only — were a foundational part of our relationship and our identities. No matter what happened, we could make each other laugh.

essay on racial bias

Channing Kennedy is an Oakland-based writer, performer, media producer and racial equity trainer. Courtesy of Channing Kennedy hide caption

It helped, also, that we were slackers with spare time, but eventually we both found callings. I started working in the social justice sector, and he gained recognition in the field of indie comics. I was proud of my new job and approached it seriously, if not gracefully. Before I took the job, I was the type of white dude who'd make casually racist comments in front of people I considered friends. Now, I had laid a new foundation for myself and was ready to undo the harm I'd done pre-wokeness.

And I was proud of him, too, if cautious. The indie comics scene is full of bravely offensive work: the power fantasies of straight white men with grievances against their nonexistent censors, put on defiant display. But he was my friend, and he wouldn't fall for that.

One day he emailed me a rough script to get my feedback. At my desk, on a break from deleting racist, threatening Facebook comments directed at my co-workers, I opened it up for a change of pace.

I got none. His script was a top-tier, irredeemable power fantasy — sex trafficking, disability jokes, gendered violence, every scene's background packed with commentary-devoid, racist caricatures. It also had a pop culture gag on top, to guarantee clicks.

I asked him why he'd written it. He said it felt "important." I suggested he shelve it. He suggested that that would be a form of censorship. And I realized this: My dear friend had created a racist power fantasy about dismembering women, and he considered it bravely offensive.

I could have said that there was nothing brave about catering to the established tastes of other straight white comics dudes. I could have dropped any number of half-understood factoids about structural racism, the finishing move of the recently woke. I could have just said the jokes were weak.

Instead, I became cruel to him, with a dedication I'd previously reserved for myself.

Over months, I redirected every bit of our old creativity. I goaded him into arguments I knew would leave him shaken and unable to work. I positioned myself as a surrogate parent (so I could tell myself I was still a concerned ally) then laughed at him. I got him to escalate. And, privately, I told myself it was me who was under attack, the one with the grievance, and I cried about how my friend was betraying me.

I wanted to erase him (I realized years later) not because his script offended me, but because it made me laugh. It was full of the sense of humor we'd spent years on — not the jokes verbatim, but the pacing, structure, reveals, go-to gags. It had my DNA and it was funny. I thought I had become a monster-slayer, but this comic was a monster with my hands and mouth.

After years as the best of friends and as the bitterest of exes, we finally had a chance to meet in person. We were little more than acquaintances with sunk costs at that point, but we met anyway. Maybe we both wanted forgiveness, or an apology, or to see if we still had some jokes. Instead, I lectured him about electoral politics and race in a bar and never smiled.

  • code switch

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Cogn Res Princ Implic
  • v.6; 2021 Dec

Systemic racism: individuals and interactions, institutions and society

Mahzarin r. banaji.

1 Harvard University, Cambridge, MA USA

Susan T. Fiske

2 Princeton University, Princeton, NJ USA

Douglas S. Massey

Systemic racism is a scientifically tractable phenomenon, urgent for cognitive scientists to address. This tutorial reviews the built-in systems that undermine life opportunities and outcomes by racial category, with a focus on challenges to Black Americans. From American colonial history, explicit practices and policies reinforced disadvantage across all domains of life, beginning with slavery, and continuing with vastly subordinated status. Racially segregated housing creates racial isolation, with disproportionate costs to Black Americans’ opportunities, networks, education, wealth, health, and legal treatment. These institutional and societal systems build-in individual bias and racialized interactions, resulting in systemic racism. Unconscious inferences, empirically established from perceptions onward, demonstrate non-Black Americans’ inbuilt associations: pairing Black Americans with negative valences, criminal stereotypes, and low status, including animal rather than human . Implicit racial biases (improving only slightly over time) imbed within non-Black individuals’ systems of racialized beliefs, judgments, and affect that predict racialized behavior. Interracial interactions likewise convey disrespect and distrust. These systematic individual and interpersonal patterns continue partly due to non-Black people’s inexperience with Black Americans and reliance on societal caricatures. Despite systemic challenges, Black Americans are more diverse now than ever, due to resilience (many succeeding against the odds), immigration (producing varied backgrounds), and intermarriage (increasing the multiracial proportion of the population). Intergroup contact can foreground Black diversity, resisting systemic racism, but White advantages persist in all economic, political, and social domains. Cognitive science has an opportunity: to include in its study of the mind the distortions of reality about individual humans and their social groups.

Introduction

Significance.

American racial biases persist over time and permeate (a) institutional structures, (b) societal structures, (c) individual mental structures, (d) everyday interaction patterns. Systemic racism operates with or without intention and with or without awareness. But because these responses are based on socially defined racial categories, they are racialized, and because they are negative, they reveal the roots of racism. At the level of most behavior, they are also controllable, even if many non-Black people rarely notice these relentless patterns. Systemic racism is a unified arrangement of racial differentiation and discrimination across generations. Understanding these formidable challenges is necessary to understand and then dismantle them. Cognitive science can illuminate the fine-grained levels of inbuilt racial bias because it has the methods and the theories to do so. Moreover, studying racial bias is interesting; it will improve the science; and it is the obvious path to ensuring a mutually respectful, peaceful society that flourishes economically, politically, and socially.

At the Editor’s invitation, this article presents the social and behavioral science of systemic racism to a cognitive science audience. The tutorial defines systemic racism, describes its origins in US history, shows how the resulting racialized societal structures have become built-in cognitive structures that propagate in social interactions, resisting change. But these very societal-cognitive-social features can also be agents for change.

Systemic racism is said to occur when racially unequal opportunities and outcomes are inbuilt or intrinsic to the operation of a society’s structures. Simply put, systemic racism refers to the processes and outcomes of racial inequality and inequity in life opportunities and treatment. Systemic racism permeates a society’s (a) institutional structures (practices, policies, climate), (b) social structures (state/federal programs, laws, culture), (c) individual mental structures (e.g., learning, memory, attitudes, beliefs, values), and (d) everyday interaction patterns (norms, scripts, habits). Systemic racism not only operates at multiple levels, it can emerge with or without animus or intention to harm and with or without awareness of its existence. Its power derives from its being integrated into a unified system of racial differentiation and discrimination that creates, governs, and adjudicates opportunities and outcomes across generations. Racism represents the biases of the powerful (Jones, 1971 ), as the biases of the powerless have little consequence (Fiske, 1993 ). 1

We highlight the “inbuilt” aspect of systemic racism to be its signature feature and the touchstone necessary to understand the nature of systemic racism and its resistance to awareness and change. We begin with the concept’s more traditional domains: institutional and societal systems. Then, given the current venue, we expand the levels of analysis to include individual mental systems that have built in those systems of inequalities. We close with the interaction of those minds in social behavior, which can either maintain or change racial systems.

Institutions and Society . As the first section explains, the term systemic racism has traditionally referred to systems that uphold racism via institutional power (Feagin, 2006 ), with stark examples of what is also called institutional racism (Jones, 1972 ) visible in inequities in housing and lending, as well as more broadly in access to finance, education, healthcare, and justice. This section focuses on the institutional level in depth, as it provides the strongest evidence of systemic racism. At an even more macro s ocietal level, however, the inbuilt aspect of systemic racism is evident in race-based demarcations created by large-scale state and federal programs, which offer levers either to increase or decrease systemic racism. To remain within the scope of the paper, we consider the structures of institutional and societal racism in a single section.

Individuals and Interactions . In tandem with the previous section, this section focuses on individual bias and interactional racism, together bringing into view the inbuilt nature of systemic racism. To expand on this inclusive view of systemic racism, we end by reviewing what we know about the individual human being, alone and interacting with others. Individuals are agentic entities, the primary actors within all systems of life and living. Their attitudes (preferences, prejudices), beliefs (stereotypes), and behaviors (discrimination) are inbuilt or intrinsically enmeshed into the foundation of the mental systems that feed systemic racism. At the individual level, “inbuilt” refers to the common psychological processes that represent race in the minds of individuals. This evidence reveals systemic race bias.

Note that, here, we use slightly different terms: Systemic Racism refers to much of the sociological, demographic, and historic material as well as anything in the psychological section that is explicit and conscious racism. Systemic Race Bias is about implicit cognition—people who may not be aware of the harm they may cause. Implicit race bias does not mean a person is a racist. In this view, keeping racism and bias separate as terms seems advisable. Others view even unexamined racism as systemic racism in its individual manifestation. Each section elaborates on the meaning of racism in that context.

Individual racial bias propagates through both face-to-face and virtual interactions within families, classrooms, playfields, and workplaces, both verbally and non-verbally. Individual minds create and consume racial representations in books, social media, and entertainment. 2 We focus here on everyday interactions that convey disrespect and distrust of Black Americans.

Why? Role for psychological science in studying systemic racism

Individual humans are the creators and consumers of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, but also the policies and practices that lie at the heart of systemic racism. Psychology as a field has historically remained silent on the topic of systemic racism, per se (e.g., Guthrie, 2004 , “Even the rat was white”; for exceptions, see: Jones, 1971 ; DuBois, 1925 ). Perhaps psychologists have regarded systemic racism to be a form of institutional racism and hence in the bailiwick of social scientists who study institutions and society, not individuals. Nonetheless, we attempt here to include individual minds and face-to-face interaction as playing a role. This goal has precedents: Early scholars who straddled disciplines, such as George Herbert Mead ( 1934 , p. 174), would likely find our attempt to be quite compatible with his stance that mind and society must be considered in intertwined fashion.

Today, psychologists are increasingly attempting to bridge the divide between the individual mind and society. Cultural psychology, for example, has attempted to analyze racism as the “budding product of psychological subjectivity and the structural foundation for dynamic reproduction of racist action” (Salter, Adams & Perez, 2018 , p. 151). This dynamic can emerge in individual racist actions (with or without awareness) that are fitted into the structure of everyday life and perpetuate systemic racism. Interpersonal interactions bridge individual and collective representations of race. Individual minds, sharing some notions about each other’s salient identities (e.g., probable race, gender, age) treat each other according to social norms, cultural habits, and cultural scripts. In the case of race, these individual mental representations and social interaction patterns rarely benefit Black participants facing Whites.

“Inbuilt”: A useful metaphor guiding the essay

There are these two fish swimming along and they happen to meet an older fish swimming the other way, who nods at them and says ’Morning, boys. How’s the water?’ And the two young fish swim on for a bit, and then eventually one of them looks over at the other and goes ‘What the hell is water?’ Wallace, 2009

The fable highlights a simple idea—that the most fundamental feature of any system may be so completely pervasive that it ceases to be perceptible or when perceptible, fails to be recognized in its true form. This paradox creates a challenge for social and behavioral scientists, who must not only generate evidence about the complexities of systemic racism, but we must also confront unthinking rejection of that evidence. Other scientists face similar challenges in documenting their own complex phenomena, such as the resistance faced by the theory of evolution or the denial of evidence about climate change.

In most cases, evidence eventually reaches a tipping point, after which it ceases to be denied and even becomes sufficiently commonplace that its previous denial itself is puzzling. An easy example is the denial of scientific evidence about the position of the earth in the solar system and its shape, with few arguments today (but not zero!) about a flat earth. However, we are far from that tipping point of knowledge and acceptance when it comes to the idea of systemic racism. This paper, then, is yet another attempt, by connecting across the individual, interactional, and institutional/societal levels, to shed light on its existence.

The obvious allegorical lesson from the fable about the fish is of course the ease of being ignorant of that which is pervasive. However, the fable also points out that not all the fish are ignorant of their surroundings. The older fish, swimming the same ocean as the young fish, seems to have figured out the truth about the substance that suffuses its environment so fully that it is imperceptible to its peers. Ignorance then, need not be the only guaranteed outcome, even when perception and awareness are hard. Hence, one section uses the term “unexamined” to describe controllable attention to or willful neglect of one’s own biases (see also Fiske, 1998 ). Social scientists commenting on resistance to socioeconomic inequality have used the term “clueless” (Williams, 2019 ), which is admittedly harsh but suggests that learning some facts would permit more evidence-based understanding. Regardless, the evidence for systemic racism, at the level of institutions and society or at the level of individuals and interactions, requires re-examining the taken-for-granted, whether the water we swim or the air we breathe.

Systemic racism: the role of institutional and societal structures

Contemporary societal racism rests on Black–White segregation, historical and current. This first substantive section presents evidence that systemic racism has long pervaded US institutional and societal systems—creating a context for the minds of individuals within these systems, enabling an omnipresent neglect. First, this section shows that continued housing segregation by race obstructs Black opportunity and mobility, perpetuating racial disparities, challenging many Black Americans in ways White Americans never experience (Massey, 2020 ). At a societal level, Black disadvantage and White advantage come in part from residential hypersegregation (Massey & Tannen, 2015 ). More than any other racial group, Whites live in racially isolated neighborhoods (Rugh & Massey, 2014 ); and in the US neighborhood segregation translates directly into school segregation (Massey & Tannen, 2016 ; Owens, 2020 ). Both segregation and local funding undermine the quality of predominantly Black schools.

To elaborate these points, this section describes the historical context for US racism, territory likely to be less familiar to cognitive scientists. Our takeaway: Systemic racism pervades US social institutions, policies, and practices; later sections show how the societal structures make into the minds of the humans within these systems.

History: segregation and systemic racism

To explain systemic racism, we start with the historical origins of race in the US—that is, the social/political/economic mechanisms that have maintained it over time. Race is baked into the history of the US going back to colonial times (Higginbotham, 1998 ; Jones, 1972 , 1997 ) and continuing through early independence when slavery was quietly written into the nation’s Constitution (Waldstreicher, 2009 ). Although the 13th, 14th, and 15th Amendments to the Constitution ended slavery and granted due process, equal protection, and voting rights to the formerly enslaved, efforts to combat systemic racism in the US faltered when Reconstruction collapsed in the disputed election of 1876, which triggered the withdrawal of federal troops from the South (Foner, 1990 ).

The absence of federal troops to enforce Black civil rights enabled states in the former Confederacy to construct a new system of racial subordination known as Jim Crow (Packard, 2003 ). It rested on a simple principle: in any social encounter, the lowest status White person was superior to the highest status Black person. By law and custom, Black voting rights were suppressed, and Black Americans were socially segregated from Whites, relegated to menial occupations, inferior schools, dilapidated housing, and deficient facilities throughout Southern society. Any challenges to the Jim Crow system, perceived or real, were met with violence, often lethal, both within and outside the legal system (Tolnay & Beck, 1995 ).

From 1876 to 1900, 90% of all African Americans lived in the South and were subject to the dictates of the repressive Jim Crow system; 83% lived in poor rural areas, occupying ramshackle dwellings clustered in small settlements in or near the plantations where they worked. Although conditions were somewhat better for the 10% of African Americans who lived outside the South (68% in cities), anti-Black prejudice was widespread, racial discrimination was common and, as in the South, the prospect of racial violence was never far away (Sugrue, 2008 ).

Before, 1900, few African Americans lived in cities, and levels of urban racial residential segregation were modest. Black workers and servants generally lived within walking distance of their workplaces, and social contact between the races was common (Massey & Denton, 1993 ). At that time, the share of Blacks among city residents was small, and they were not perceived to be a threat to White hegemony, obviating the need for spatial segregation. The Great Black Migration of the twentieth century changed this status quo and transformed race relations in the US, making race truly a national rather than regional issue (Lemann, 1991 ). This transformation also created a new system of racial subordination based on Black residential segregation.

Between 1900 and 1970, millions of African Americans left the rural South in search of better lives in industrializing cities throughout the nation. As a result of this migration, by 1970 nearly half of all African Americans had come to live outside the South, 90% in urban areas (Farley & Allen, 1987 ). It was during this period of Black urbanization that the ghetto emerged as a structural feature of American urbanism, making Black residential segregation into the linchpin of a new system of racial stratification that prevailed throughout the US irrespective of region (Pettigrew, 1979 ).

Black out-migration from the South began slowly at first, but accelerated after 1914, when the onset of the First World War curtailed the arrival of workers from Europe. It accelerated again after 1917, when the US entered the war, boosting labor demand as conscription drew workers out of the labor force. The imposition of strict immigration restrictions in 1921 and 1924 guaranteed that Black workers and their families would continue to pour into cities during the economic boom of the 1920s (Wilkerson, 2010 ). The entry of ever-larger cohorts of impoverished Black laborers and sharecroppers into the nation’s cities unnerved White urbanites, prompting them to organize collectively by creating “neighborhood improvement associations.” These organizations pressured landlords not to rent to Black tenants and tried to convince Black home seekers that it was in their best interest to locate elsewhere, using persuasion and payoffs when possible but resorting to violence when these blandishments failed (Massey & Denton, 1993 ).

As the number of incoming Black migrants continued to rise despite these efforts, White city residents demanded that politicians act to “do something” about the perceived “Black invasion.” Officials in smaller towns and cities responded by enacting “sundown laws” that required all Blacks to leave town by sunset (Loewen, 2018 ). In large cities, legislators passed municipal ordinances that confined Black residents to a specific set of already disadvantaged neighborhoods and excluded them from all others. These ordinances were the functional equivalent of South Africa’s Group Areas Act, which underlay the establishment of that country’s apartheid system in, 1948. These ordinances were widely copied and were spreading rapidly from city to city when, in 1917, the Supreme Court declared them to be unconstitutional (Massey & Denton, 1993 ). Sundown laws, however, were never challenged in court and remained in force well into the Civil Rights Era.

The end of legally mandated neighborhood segregation in cities occurred just as Black migration surged in the aftermath of America’s entry into the First World War. The sudden influx of workers caused existing areas of Black settlement to fill up rapidly and eventually overflow into adjacent White areas, where the arrivals met with increasingly violent resistance. The violence peaked in the late teens as anti-Black race riots swept through the nation’s cities, culminating in the Great Chicago Race Riot of 1919 (Tuttle, 1970 ). Even established Black neighborhoods were not safe, as evidenced by the Tulsa Massacre of 1921, in which the prosperous Black neighborhood of Greenwood was systematically attacked and razed by mobs of White vigilantes, leaving thousands homeless and dozens, perhaps hundreds, killed (Madigan, 2001 ).

Shocked by the wanton destruction of property, the real estate industry moved to institutionalize racial discrimination in housing markets and assert control over the process of racial change in cities (Massey & Denton, 1993 ). In 1924, the National Association of Real Estate Brokers adopted a code of ethics stating that “a Realtor should never be instrumental in introducing into a neighborhood a character of property or occupancy, members of any race or nationality, or any individuals whose presence will clearly be detrimental to property values in that neighborhood” (Helper, 1969 , p. 201). In 1927, the Chicago Real Estate Board devised a model racial covenant to block the entry of Blacks into White neighborhoods and offered it to other cities for adoption throughout the country (Massey & Denton, 1993 ). A racial covenant is a private contract in which property owners within a defined geographic area collectively agree not to rent or sell to African Americans. Once approved by a majority of property owners, the contract became enforceable, and violators could be sued in civil court.

As the real estate industry gradually assumed control of racial change in urban areas, racial violence abated and neighborhood transitions from White to Black came to be managed professionally by realtors who sought to minimize confrontation and maximize profits. As Black migration continued throughout the 1920s, recognized Black neighborhoods steadily increased in density as housing units were divided and subdivided. Basements, garages, attics, and even closets were converted into rental units. Eventually, however, no more living space could be squeezed into the confines of the existing ghetto. Realtors then conspired to move the residential color line, selecting an adjacent neighborhood for racial transition and initiating an institutionalized process known as “block busting” (Philpott, 1978 ).

Realtors began the process by choosing a few poor Black families just arrived from the rural South and obviously unused to city ways to be placed strategically into selected units within the targeted neighborhood. Agents then moved through the neighborhood block by block warning residents of a pending Black “invasion.” Panic selling ensued, enabling realtors to purchase homes cheaply for subdivision into smaller apartments, which were then leased at inflated rents to African Americans desperate for living space. Owing to these institutionalized practices, Black segregation levels steadily climbed through the 1920s and ghetto areas gradually expanded their boundaries through the profitable management of neighborhood racial turnover by realtors (Massey & Denton, 1993 ).

The exclusively private auspices of Black residential segregation ended with the onset of the Great Depression in 1929. When Franklin Roosevelt came to power with his New Deal in 1933, the nation was in the midst of a catastrophic banking crisis. Millions of middle-class homeowners had lost jobs and were in danger of defaulting on their mortgages, putting both their homes and their bankers at financial risk. In response, the Roosevelt Administration created the Home Owners Loan Corporation (HOLC) to help middle class homeowners refinance their mortgages using long-term, federally insured, low-interest loans (Jackson, 1985 ). Together the federal guarantees and extended amortization periods reduced monthly mortgage payments to affordable levels, saving both the banks and the homeowners from financial losses through foreclosure.

To qualify for the federal guarantees, however, HOLC loans had to meet certain government-mandated criteria. In addition to low interest rates, minimal down payments, and long amortization periods, lenders were obliged to consider the riskiness of the neighborhoods in which properties were located. To this end, HOLC officials worked with local realtors and bankers to create a series of Residential Security Maps for use in cities throughout the nation. These maps color-coded neighborhoods according to their creditworthiness. Green indicated a safe investment, yellow indicated caution, and red indicated excessive risk and hence ineligibility for HOLC lending. Black neighborhoods were invariably coded red, along with adjacent neighborhoods perceived to be at risk of Black settlement (Rothstein, 2017 ).

The HOLC lending program only helped the minority of families that already owned homes, however, and in order to spread housing wealth to a wider population and create jobs in the real estate and construction industries, in 1934 the Roosevelt Administration created a much larger loan program under the Federal Housing Authority. The FHA offered long-term loans to prospective home buyers , not just owners. As before, federally guaranteed loans had to meet federally mandated criteria, which evinced a strong anti-urban bias. Specifically, they excluded from eligibility all multiunit buildings, attached dwellings, row houses, and structures containing a business. These provisions effectively restricted FHA loans to single family houses on large lots, thus channeling housing investment away from central cities toward vacant land on the urban fringes (Jackson, 1985 ).

Reflecting the prejudices of the realtors, bankers, and builders who helped to design the program, FHA underwriters were also required to make use of the HOLC’s Residential Security Maps, formally institutionalizing the practice of redlining in real estate and banking and systematically cutting off investment in Black neighborhoods for decades to come. The FHA Underwriter’s Manual explicitly stated that “if a neighborhood is to retain stability, it is necessary that properties shall continue to be occupied by the same social and racial classes.” In addition to requiring the use of Residential Security Maps, the manual went on to advocate the use of racial covenants to protect FHA-insured properties. When a parallel loan program was created in the Veterans Administration by the 1944 Servicemen’s Readjustment Act, it adopted the FHA’s racialized practices and procedures (Katznelson, 2006 ).

The anti-urban biases and discriminatory practices built into federal loan programs had little effect on housing patterns during the 1930s and 1940s owing to the tiny amount of new residential construction that occurred during the Great Depression and Second World War. In the postwar period, however, FHA and VA lending drove forward a massive wave of suburban home construction that made new homes widely accessible to White but not Black households. Given high rents and home prices in central cities owing to the influx of workers during the war years, in the late 1940s and early 1950s it was cheaper to buy a brand-new house in the suburbs than to rent an apartment in the city (Massey & Denton, 1993 ).

The end result was a government-subsidized mass exodus of middle and working class White families from central cities to suburbs, creating a distinctly American urban configuration of Black cities surrounded by White suburbs. The homes left behind by the departing Whites seeking their piece of the American Dream in the suburbs were quickly occupied by Black in-movers coming to the city to take jobs in the still-vibrant urban manufacturing sector. Neighborhood turnover accelerated, and the nation’s urban Black ghettos rapidly expanded, both demographically and geographically (Massey & Denton, 1993 ).

Although neighborhood transitions in the 1950s and 1960s improved Black access to housing in the short term, in the long term the neighborhoods turned into poverty traps. Because of redlining and racial discrimination built into housing and credit markets by federal policies and private practices, once a neighborhood became Black, it was cut off from investment, ensuring that its housing stock and business infrastructure would progressively deteriorate. It also left the Black middle class without a means to finance the purchase of homes, and predatory lenders stepped into the resulting void.

Drawing on their own capital, these lenders purchased homes and then offered to “sell” them to middle class Black families by means of Loan Installment Contracts (Satter, 2009 ). LICs were essentially rent-to-own schemes with high interest rates, bloated monthly payments, and no property rights or transfer of title until the final contract payment was made. Any missed payment could bring about immediate eviction by the property owner, no matter how long the aspiring family had been making payments under the contract.

Other predatory investors also purchased ghetto properties to become landlords, subdividing them into ever-smaller units and leasing them to poor and working class Black tenants at inflated rents (Massey & Denton, 1993 ). Whether city housing was being sold under an installment contract or rented on usurious terms, however, the absentee owners could not themselves get loans to offset depreciation or purchase insurance policies to protect their properties, creating a strong financial incentive for landlords to defer maintenance, minimize capital investment, and extract high rents as long as possible until the properties deteriorated to the point of becoming uninhabitable.

As Black ghettos expanded geographically during the 1950s and 1960s in cities such as New York, Chicago, Philadelphia, Detroit, Cleveland, and St. Louis, they ultimately came to encroach on zones in which White elites had place-bound investments in universities, hospitals, museums, and business districts. In desperation, local politicians and civic leaders turned to state and federal agencies for help. Drawing on funding from the National Housing Act, they created locally controlled Urban Renewal Authorities with the power of eminent domain, thereby enabling White interests to gain control of the Black neighborhoods threatening their place-bound investments (Bauman, 1987 ; Hirsch, 1983 ). Once in control of the land, they evicted the residents, razed their homes, and demolished neighborhood businesses, replacing them either with large-scale middle-class housing projects or institutional developments that strategically blocked the expansion of the ghetto toward the threatened White properties, prompting James Baldwin to quip that “urban renewal means Negro removal” (Dickinson, 1963 ).

Because of a “one-for-one rule” embedded within the National Housing Act, for every unit of housing torn down in the name of renewal, planners had to identify another unit into which the displaced tenants could theoretically move. To satisfy this rule, local elites once again turned to the federal government, garnering additional funds authorized by the National Housing Act to construct large public housing projects for families displaced by renewal. Given that the displaced families were Black, it was politically impossible to build the housing project in a White district, so another Black neighborhood was targeted for renewal and torn down to build dense collections of high-rise projects that now had to house two neighborhood’s worth of displaced families (Massey & Denton, 1993 ).

This pairing of urban renewal and public housing did not itself increase the level of Black residential segregation (Bickford & Massey, 1991 ). Segregation levels were already high in the cities where this pairing occurred; but it did dramatically increase the spatial concentration of poverty within the ghetto by replacing relatively class-diverse Black neighborhoods and business districts with tightly packed blocks of high-rise projects in which being poor was a criterion for entry, yielding neighborhood poverty rates of 90% or more (Massey & Kanaiaupuni, 1993 ).

By 1970, high levels of Black residential segregation were universal throughout metropolitan America (Massey & Denton, 1993 ). 3 As of 1970, 61% of Black Americans living in US metropolitan areas lived under a regime of hypersegregation (Massey & Tannen, 2015 ), a circumstance unique to Americans. Although in theory, segregation should have withered away after the Civil Rights Era, it has not. In 2010, the average index of Black–White segregation remained high and a third of all Black metropolitan residents continued to live in hypersegregated areas (Massey & Tannen, 2015 ). This reality prevails despite the outlawing of racial discrimination in housing (the 1968 Fair Housing Act) and lending (the 1974 Equal Credit Opportunity Act and the 1977 Community Reinvestment Act).

Why does modern segregation persist, despite Whites’ reported racial attitudes improving?

Accompanying these legislative changes was a pronounced shift in White racial attitudes. In the early 1960s, more than 60% of White Americans agreed that Whites have a right to keep Blacks out of their neighborhoods. By the 1980s, however, the percentage had dropped to 13% (Schuman et al., 1998 ). The fact that discrimination is illegal, and White support for segregation has plummeted, begs the question of why segregation persists. The reasons are multiple.

First, although the Fair Housing Act banned discrimination in the rental and sale of housing, enforcement mechanisms in the original legislation were eliminated as part of a compromise to secure the bill’s passage (Metcalf, 1988 ). Federal authorities were likewise granted only limited powers to enforce the Equal Credit Opportunity Act and the Community Reinvestment Act (Massey & Denton, 1993 ).

Although overt discrimination in housing and lending has clearly declined in response to legislation, covert discrimination continues. Rental and sales agents today are less likely to respond to emails from people with stereotypically Black names (Carpusor & Loges, 2006 ; Hanson & Hawley, 2011 ) or to reply to phone messages left by speakers who “sound Black” (Massey & Fischer, 2004 ; Massey & Lundy, 2001 ). A recent meta-analysis of 16 experimental housing audit studies and 19 lending analyses conducted since 1970 revealed that sharp racial differentials in the number of units recommended by realtors and inspected by clients have persisted and that racial gaps in loan denial rates and borrowing cost have barely changed in 40 years (Quillian, Lee, & Honoré, 2020 ).

Audit studies, conducted across the social and behavioral sciences, include a subset of resume studies in which researchers send the same resume out to apply for jobs, but change just one item: the candidate’s name is Lisa Smith or Lakisha Smith. Then, they wait to see who gets the callback. The bias is clear: employers avoid “Black-sounding” names (Bertrand & Mullainathan, 2004 ). In fact, in both Milwaukee’s and New York City’s low-wage job market, Black applicants with no criminal background were called back with the same frequency or less as White applicants just released from prison (Pager, 2003 ; Pager, Western & Bonikowski, 2009 ).

That is, in the minds of hiring managers whose mental make-up is expected to be no different than the readers of this article, a White felon is equivalent to a Black non-felon. The same housing application, the same bank loan application, the same health data, the same behavior, lead to different outcomes depending on the race of the applicant, even though the decision-makers believe they are paying attention to the merits of the case and explicitly not to race, which most decision makers in these studies regard to be irrelevant to the decision.

What makes the problem of systemic racism so perverse is that “good people” with no explicit expression of we would call “racism” are the contributors to such decisions that produce widespread and unnoticed bias, resulting in systemic racism (Banaji & Greenwald, 2013 ). Racial discrimination continues because, although White support for Black segregation may have declined in principle, Whites nonetheless continue to harbor negative racial stereotypes about Black people , which limit their tolerance for integration in practice. Indeed, the willingness of Whites to enter or remain in a neighborhood declines steadily as the percentage of Black neighbors rises (Charles, 2003 ; Emerson, Chai & Yancey, 2001 ). And negative racial stereotyping of Black Americans strongly predicts White opposition to government efforts to enforce Black civil rights (Bobo, Charles, Krysan & Simmons, 2012 ).

In White American social cognition, as later sections elaborate, racial biases remain entrenched both explicitly (Moberg, Krysan & Christianson, 2019 ) and implicitly (Eberhardt, 2019 ). This extends to preferred neighborhoods : Residential searches are inevitably embedded within racialized expectations about neighborhoods and homes that reflect the racially segregated world that most Americans inhabit (Krysan & Crowder, 2017 ). The “correlated characteristics heuristic” relies on a single salient neighborhood trait—in this case racial composition—to represent an area’s acceptability. In White social cognition, the mere presence of Blacks denotes lower property values, higher crime rates, and struggling schools, irrespective of what the objective neighborhood conditions are (Krysan, Couper, Farley & Forman, 2009 ; Quillian & Pager, 2001 , 2010 ). Although Whites in surveys and interviews say they welcome the presence of Black neighbors, in practice Whites avoid neighborhoods containing more than a few Blacks and confine their searches to overwhelmingly White residential areas exhibiting White percentages well above those they report in describing their “ideal” neighborhood on surveys (Krysan & Crowder, 2017 ).

Although rarely admitted, explicit prejudice against Black Americans has hardly disappeared. Google search frequencies on the epithet “nigger” for different metropolitan areas strongly predicted an area’s level of Black residential segregation (Rugh & Massey, 2014 ). This index of explicit racism also strongly predicts the degree to which a city’s suburbs are covered by restrictive density zoning regimes (Massey and Rugh ( 2018 ), a key proximate cause of both racial and class segregation (Rothwell & Massey, 2009 , 2010 ). Owing to the persistence of discrimination, Black Americans are far less able that other Americans to translate their income attainments into residential mobility, greatly compromising their ability to access more integrated and favored neighborhoods (Massey & Denton, 1985 ). As of 2010, the most affluent Black Americans were still more segregated from Whites than the poorest Hispanics (Intrator, Tannen & Massey, 2016 ).

No other group in the history of the US has ever experienced such intense residential segregation in so many areas and over such a long period of time (Massey & Denton, 1993 ; Rugh & Massey, 2014 ). Systemic racism in federal housing policies (Katznelson, 2006 ), real estate (Helper, 1969 ), banking (Ross & Yinger, 2002 ), and insurance (Orren, 1974 ) has ensured a vicious cycle of racial turnover and neighborhood deterioration for most of the past century. As a result, many Black Americans have been compelled to live in societally isolated, economically disadvantaged, physically deteriorated neighborhoods produced and sustained by powerful external forces beyond their ability to control, the precise embodiment of systemic racism.

Because of racial residential segregation and the blocked mobility and spatial concentration of poverty it produces, neighborhoods have become the key nexus for the transmission of Black socioeconomic disadvantage over the life course and across the generations (Sharkey, 2013 ). Half of all Black Americans have lived in the poorest quartile of urban neighborhoods for two consecutive generations, compared with just 7% of Whites, a gap that cannot be explained by individual or family characteristics.

Whereas in the 1960s Black poverty was transmitted across generations by the inheritance of race and the discrimination and exclusion that came with it (Duncan, 1969 ), in the twenty-first century Black poverty is transmitted by the inheritance of place and the concentrated poverty it entails (Massey, 2013 ; Massey & Brodmann, 2014 ; Peterson & Krivo, 2010 ; Sampson, 2012 ; Sharkey, 2013 ). Black disadvantage with respect to income and social mobility is explained almost entirely by the poor neighborhood circumstances they experience (Chetty, Hendren, Jones & Porter, 2020 ; Massey & Brodmann, 2014 ). Racial residential segregation has become linchpin for systemic racism in the US in the twenty-first century (Massey, 2016 , 2020 ).

Discussions of segregation typically highlight how it operates to increase the social isolation of Blacks, but in fact it does more to isolate Whites, who are by far the most spatially isolated group in the US. In 2010, the average Black metropolitan resident lived in a neighborhood that was 45% Black, but the average White metropolitan resident occupied a neighborhood that was 74% White (Massey, 2018 ), and in suburbs the figure rose to 80% (Massey & Tannen, 2017 ). As a result, the advantages of segregation to Whites and the disadvantages to Blacks are invisible to most White Americans.

Feagin ( 1999 , p. 79), put this paradox into perspective by relating the experience of a British immigrant’s confrontation with the realities of race in the US:

Some time after English writer Henry Fairlie emigrated to the USA in the mid-1960s, he visited Thomas Jefferson’s Monticello plantation and took the standard tour. When the White guide asked for questions, Fairlie inquired, “Where did he keep his slaves?” Fairlie reports that the other tourists looked at him in disturbed silence, while the guide “swallowed hard” and said firmly that “the slaves’ quarters are not included in the official tour.” (Fairlie, 1985 .) Housing segregation, and the systemic racism it reveals, are still not on the official tour.”

Two decades later, the question we must answer is whether we are willing, as scientists and citizens, to put housing segregation—and all the other institutions that do so much to dictate the vicissitudes of Black life—on the official tour of the USA.

Systemic racial bias: the role of mental structures and resulting social interactions

We began with institutions and society. Now, we move to individual minds surrounded and shaped by these societal structures. Next, we then move to interacting minds, which further perpetuate societal and individual racial distinctions. Racial bias at each level supports bias at the other levels, creating a racist system.

To understand individual mental structures, we start with unconscious inference, identified by Helmholtz, and its heir, implicit bias, most relevantly as expressed by Whites associating Black racial cues with negative concepts. Socially motivated (mis)perception goes one stage earlier to bias information seeking and interpretation. More specific links among racial bias in perceiving physiognomy, linked to dehumanizing associations, and aggressive behavior close this first section on the individual.

Unconscious inference

Among the intellectuals who contributed to the emergence of experimental psychology as an independent discipline in the nineteenth century was the German polymath, Herman von Helmholtz, whose numerous contributions to science include the concept of “ Unbewuste Schluss ” or “ unconscious inference .” Helmholtz’s concept was simple, but its implications are profound, even more so today with recent advances in the mind and brain sciences. Given the complexity of just the visual world, how are humans to represent it based on their individual-level, meager sensory and perceptual system, which entails the shunting of packets of data from the world outside, through the eyes and into the brain? Helmholtz offered two ideas. First, perception is not veridical, given the complexity of the world and the rudimentary nature of the minds attempting to make sense of it. Second, as implied by the word inference , what one deduces from the evidence provided by the senses is not a replica of what is out there. Rather, mental representations of the physical world are mere approximations.

Whittling the self-esteem of Homo sapiens down further, Helmholtz went on to say that perception is not controllable, but rather that it unfolds automatically. He used a commonplace example to make this point. We know that it is not the Sun that rises, but rather that the Earth revolves around it. But when we sit on our porch at sunrise, and look toward the horizon, we incontrovertibly experience ourselves as being fixed, and the Sun, however bulky, pushing itself up to meet us. To say about the Sun that “it rises” is completely inaccurate yet completely compelling. That incorrect perception is not something over which we have choice. To think otherwise is to delude ourselves.

Helmholtz’s two ideas contained in the phrase “unconscious inference,” with many additional levels of social complexity, summarizes the challenge when we confront systemic racism. On the one hand, we “know” the facts about an economy purportedly mounted on free labor for 250 years, the undelivered promise of 40 acres and a mule, the failure of Reconstruction, the resistance to desegregation, the history of redlining and gerrymandering, a history of unequal access to education, jobs, housing, finance, healthcare, and a lack of equal protection under the law. On the other hand, the limited sensory, perceptual, learning, and memory systems of humans set up a built-in blindness and automatic inferences that generate the illusions that, for instance, White people experience more discrimination than Black people (Norton & Sommers, 2011 ). Or, if Black Americans have any challenges, they have created their own situation in America today (Pettigrew, 1979 ) and therefore are responsible for getting themselves out of that situation. Not that minorities have no illusions, but the illusions of the higher-status group have more consequences because they usually also have more power.

The features of human minds that feed into the production of systemic racism come in two forms: ordinary errors of perception, attention, learning, memory, and reasoning that are the hallmarks of all thinking systems with human-like intelligence. In addition, we add another level of theorizing familiar to psychologists, that of motivated reasoning , the idea that our preferences, goals, and desires can bias our reasoning and lead to prejudicial decisions and outcomes (Fiske & Taylor, 2021 ; Kunda, 1990 ).

Another hallmark of human cognition is the phenomenon of loss aversion , the finding human beings much prefer avoiding losses to acquiring equivalent gains (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979 ). Even as White Americans resist and deny the reality of systemic racism, they nonetheless feel the loss of White privilege and social status quite keenly, creating powerful resentments that motivate them to reason away the potential existence of systemic racism (Craig & Richeson, 2014 ; Parker, 2021 ).

Implicit racial bias

Beginning in the 1980s, psychologists began to document a puzzling result. Individuals who claimed to have no racial animus showed evidence of negative attitudes and stereotypes toward Black Americans (Devine, 1989 ; Dovidio & Gaertner, 1986 ). Soon, the hunt for methods to better access “implicit bias” (as contrasted with standard, explicit bias measured in surveys) was underway, with specific calls for the invention of better technologies that could bypass conscious awareness or conscious control (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995 ).

One such measure, the Implicit Association Test (IAT), has demonstrated a wide array of group evaluative associations. Typically, people can pair own-group cues faster with positive concepts, and other-group cues faster with negative ones—compared with vice versa. For example, White and other non-Black Americans show robust race bias in their inability to associate “good” and “bad” equally rapidly with the social categories Black and White. The IAT has attracted considerable attention (see Greenwald et al., 2020 , for best practices, reliable effects, and ongoing investigations). A public online location, since 1998, has provided data from millions of tests taken by volunteer participants at http://www.implicit.harvard.edu . Several signature results have replicated multiple times with large samples over time:

  • Race bias is consistently visible in the data.
  • A small positive correlation between stated and implicit race attitudes exists, but the two are largely dissociated, i.e., many of those who report being neutral (no negative explicit attitudes toward Black or White Americans), do carry implicit associations of Black + bad and White + good to a larger extent than White + bad and Black + good. This result prompts us to yet again note that the term “racism” has been used by contemporary psychologists to refer to conscious forms of race prejudice and to emphasize its semi-independence from less conscious or implicit forms of race bias. To make this distinction clear, researchers who study implicit race bias have gone to great lengths to reserve the term racism to only refer to conscious expressions of racial animus. Our usage of the term systemic racism in this article is undertaken is in the interest of including all levels of analysis (individual, institutional, societal) and all forms, from the most explicit to the most implicit. The result of a low correlation between explicit racism and implicit race bias makes the point empirically that the two are not the same. Of course, implicit race bias feeds into what may become racism, and for this reason it is best to think about implicit race bias as the roots of racism, not the above ground, visible structure. Implicit race bias also results from systemic racism.
  • Asian Americans show the same pattern as White Americans, even though as a third-party group in response to a Black–White test, they might be assumed to have neutrality. From the point of view of systemic racism, this is an example of what it means to live in a system of inequity at all levels. Even third-party groups will acquire negative and positive attitudes toward groups that are not their own.
  • Black Americans express strong positive feelings toward their own group but on the measure of implicit cognition, they show no preference for their own group, with scores of almost any sample of Black Americans showing relative neutrality, i.e., equal association of good and bad for Black and White Americans. This absence of ingroup-favoring attitudes—juxtaposed with the ingroup-favoring lack of neutrality in all other groups in the same society—is open to various interpretations, from moral balance to internalized racism to astute pragmatism; all await other data.
  • Tests of anti-gay bias revealed it to be quite high in 2007 but steadily dropping off (by 64% since 2013) to be at an all-time low today. By comparison, anti-Black bias has dropped, but to a much lesser extent, by about 25% (Charlesworth & Banaji, in press). A 25% drop-off in race bias is not insignificant, and although the genders differ in magnitude of bias, both men and women are losing bias at equal speed. Although all demographic groups are changing, young Americans are changing faster than older Americans, suggesting that the world they inhabit is signaling a less biased set of attitudes.

Together, these data point to the individual manifestation of systemic racial bias, hidden from view but robustly present. However, psychologists have also gone beyond such demonstrations of basic cognitive associations as markers of implicit mental content to show that individual and institutional change is possible if the will to create change exists.

Socially motivated (mis)perception

The idea of motivated reasoning or motivated cognition gathers several useful ideas to understand how individual humans shape and even distort perception to deal with real or perceived threats to self. Kunda ( 1990 ), for example, posited that the individual need for accuracy is thwarted by the demand to reach a conclusion prior to the evidence being satisfactorily in place and that one’s goals and motives often drive decisions. These decisions reveal many identifiable biases that emerge to weaken the orientation toward accuracy (see Fiske & Taylor, 2021 ).

With more direct focus on motivated reasoning as it concerns social change, Kay et al., ( 2009 ) presented empirical evidence for a motivated tendency to view things as they are and conclude that such a state of affairs exists because it is reasonable and even representative of how things ought to be. The connection to systemic racism is quite clear, as the authors further demonstrate that motivated cognition exists in the interest of justifying sociopolitical systems that maintain inequality and resist change. People justify the status quo, preferring stability especially if they are privileged, but even if not (Jost & Banaji, 1994 ). Groups in a secure position show the cultural equivalent of inertia, seeking stability, but groups on the move express inertia as continuing to move (e.g., acquiring mainstream standing) (Zárate et al., 2019 ).

Two substantive theoretical accounts undergird these ideas as they concern complex interactions of within-person and across-person phenomena such as systemic racism. First, Sidanius and Pratto’s ( 1999 ) Theory of Social Dominance offers evolutionary and cultural evidence to support the idea that hierarchies are an almost obligatory feature of human social groups. A related but independent idea may be found in Jost’s System Justification Theory (Jost, 2020 ), which explicitly makes the case that individuals will sacrifice self and group interest in order to maintain larger “systems” of social arrangements and work to keep them in place. The reason, Jost argues, is that such a motivation serves to meet deep psychological needs for certainty, security, and acceptance by others. The overarching social structure is important to protect because if it is stable, then all within it will be safe, including those disadvantaged by established hierarchies.

Perception of phenotypes, deadly associations, and system-maintaining behavior

With regard to perceptions of race, the mere categorization of someone as Black shifts perceptions of their phenotype. For example, a series of experiments documented that people’s knowledge about race phenotypes drives perception of lightness of the skin tone (Levin & Banaji, 2006 ). In other words, experiments held skin-tone constant and varied only the features, from Afrocentric to Eurocentric; this variation in features shifts perception of skin tone, such that Afrocentric faces are viewed to be darker skinned than Eurocentric ones, despite the same gray-scale tone.

Skin tone and features are critical cues to make life and death decisions, especially in ambiguous situations that are often present in so many interactions between police and Black citizens. In simulations of police-citizen encounters, people are more likely to “shoot” unarmed Black men than otherwise equally unarmed White men (Correll, Wittenbrink, Park, Judd, & Goyle, 2010 ). Black men with more phenotypically Black features are more likely to receive the death penalty for murdering a White person, holding constant the features of the crime (Eberhardt, 2019 ). The phenotypicality effect extends even to Whites with Afrocentric features (Blair, Judd, & Chapleau, 2004 ). Judgments of criminality can be primed by a Black face (Eberhardt, 2019 ).

And there’s more: the race–crime association overlaps the dehumanizing association of Black faces with great ape faces, that Staples ( 2018 ) called the “racist trope that won’t die”; Goff, Eberhardt, Williams and Jackson ( 2008 ) provide evidence from policing that links apes and Black people, from the first moments of perception to the radio dispatch and other media, with systemic implications. In more recent work, Morehouse et al., ( 2021 ) have shown that White Americans associate White with human and Black, Asian, and Latinx with animal with greater ease than the opposite pairing (White with animal), regardless of the category of animal (generic or specific). Implicit racial biases (Whites favoring Whites) are consequential, correlating with judged trustworthiness and economic investment (Stanley, Sokol-Hessner, Banaji & Phelps, 2011 ).

More recently, Kurdi et al., ( 2021 ) measured attitudes toward a phenotypic feature that happens to be a dominant perceptual marker of race, Afrocentric and Eurocentric types of hair. First participants took an IAT measuring their implicit attitude toward Black women with natural or straightened hair. Then, subjects read a summary of a real legal case involving a corporation that fired a Black employee for refusing to change her natural hair ( Equal Employment Opportunity Commission v. Catastrophe Management Solutions , 2016). The more negative the implicit attitude toward Afrocentric hair, the greater the sympathy with the corporation’s position rather than the plaintiff’s position in the legal case.

A relatively new approach to racial associations comes with the promise of epitomizing the term “systemic” in systemic racism. These are studies of large language corpora that are now possible using machine learning approaches to natural language. With the increasing availability of trained datasets—including large samples of the language of the Internet (content archives continuously collected by the nonprofit Common Crawl) or specific trained datasets of media such as books, TV shows, etc.—allow measuring the extent to which language contains attitudes and beliefs about Black and White Americans across time. Charlesworth and Banaji (in preparation) analyzed data from Google Books from 1800 to 1990. Setting aside the data from older books to focus on whether bias is present in the language today, these are the traits most associated with Black Americans (and not with White Americans) in the late twentieth century: earthy, lonely, sensual, cruel, lifeless, deceitful, meek, rebellious, headstrong, lazy . By contrast, these are the traits associated with White Americans (and not with Black Americans): critical, decisive, hostile, friendly, polite, able, diplomatic, belligerent, understanding, confident . Other work in natural language processing (NLP) sorts adjectives into 13 stereotype-content dictionaries (Nicolas, Bai, & Fiske, 2021 ). The above adjectives convey ambivalent reactions to Black Americans on several dimensions, but notably neglect competence; Whites in contrast feature several competence adjectives. NLP allows efficient analysis of language in the culture or in spontaneous, open-ended descriptions (Nicolas, Bai, & Fiske, under review). 4

Words have an important role to play. People often express surprise about implicit biases in the minds of individuals who have no intent to harbor them. Considering how and why it occurs—plausible mechanisms—may prove convincing. One causal candidate is language , the predominant way humans communicate and express themselves. Words undertake much of the labor of creating racism in thoughts and feelings that are reflected in speech. Machine learning approaches to understanding racial bias in language will likely be a critical method to objectively uncover how words, spoken and written, create systemic racism. That is, linguistic patterns connect groups with valenced concepts, and the repeated pairings create associations. Without awareness, language produces the inbuilt in the architecture of social cognition (as an example, the NLP stereotype-dimensions dictionaries capture more than 80% of spontaneous stereotype content; Nicolas, Bai, & Fiske, under review).

From cognitive racial bias to aggregate racialized behavior

Individual implicit attitudes have been repeatedly shown to predict behavior; Kurdi et al. ( 2019 ) offer the largest number of studies included in a meta-analysis to date. However, as the authors note, the actual attitude–behavior relationship is marred by the poor quality of many studies, especially given the lack of psychometric control over the predicted behavior. Among the controversies that have marked this work is an intriguing idea put forth by Payne, Vuletich and Lundberg ( 2017 ), who proposed that the small correlations between individual attitude and behavior must be acknowledged as a function of what they call the “bias of crowds,” the idea that an individual’s behavior is determined by the larger social context in which that individual exists. A number of studies have appeared recently to challenge the idea that individual attitude–behavior correlations is the right place to be looking. That the actual correlation between implicit attitude and behavior is larger than it may have appeared has been revealed in a series of studies that predict behavior at the aggregate level by using aggregate IAT scores by region, such as metropolitan areas, counties, and states. Charlesworth and Banaji ( 2021 ) reviewed these studies to demonstrate more substantive relationships between IAT racial bias and consequential social outcomes.

For example, the studies reviewed reveal that the greater the implicit bias against Blacks in a region (using average IAT scores of a region) the greater is the lethal use of force by police, the greater the Black American deaths from circulatory diseases, the lower is spending on Medicaid disability programs (more likely to assist Black Americans), the greater the Black–White gap in infant low birth weight and preterm births, the greater the Black–White gap in school disciplining (suspension, law enforcement referrals, expulsions, in-school arrests), the Black–White gap in standardized testing scores (3rd–8th grade for math and English), and lower upward mobility.

To grasp the meaning of systemic racism as it exists at the individual level within larger society, not just in a single moment by across time, a study by Payne, Vuletich and Brown-Iannuzzi ( 2019 ) is illustrative. Their analysis of IAT data today yields strong correlations with the ratio of enslaved to free people in the southern US in 1860. States with a larger ratio in 1860 are the states with greater race bias today, 160 years later (r = 0.64). This correlation is much larger in magnitude than even the correlation between regional IAT race bias and Black American representation across the US (r = 0.32). As Charlesworth and Banaji ( 2021 ) note, “the result also suggests that today’s Americans who live in regions with greater historical legacies of slavery must be acquiring the particles of race bias embedded in the social atmosphere. Systemic discrimination is a useful term in this case as it helps capture the pervasiveness of race bias as it extends across both space and time.”

Summary. As explicit bias decreased, measured forms of implicit bias have persisted, potentially attributable to racial segregation. White Americans have limited direct experience with Black Americans, so cultural associations substitute for more individuated impressions. Implicit associations of “Black-bad” and “White-good” are weakening, but far from neutral. Meanwhile, socially motivated (mis)perception favors these system-justifying biases. Together, they support a syndrome linking racial phenotypes, deadly associations, and system-maintaining behavior. Further, cognitive racial biases underpin aggregate racialized behavior. These are some cognitive-motivational mechanisms of systemic racism. Other mechanisms involve everyday interactions that perpetuate bias. In particular, predictable patterns of disrespect and distrust maintain the interpersonal racial divide.

Racialized social interactions

Face-to-face behavior propagates bias. Individuals carry racial biases into their social settings largely by interacting with others. Repeated patterns of behavior that differ by race are, at a minimum, racialized (defined by race) and often experienced as racist. Individual racial biases, enacted in daily life, perpetuate bias, which then links the individual to the norms, scripts, and habits that constitute the social system. Interpersonal interaction conveys bias, intentionally or not. In scores of studies, White Americans distance themselves from Black interaction partners, express non-verbal discomfort, and avoid them (e.g., Dovidio, Kawakami & Gaertner, 2002 ; Richeson & Shelton, 2007 ; Word, Zanna & Cooper, 1974 ). In the aggregate, these patterns constitute the concrete manifestations of a racially biased social system.

We have already seen White people’s generically negative default associations with Black Americans, linking them to crime (untrustworthy) and to animals (incompetent). These reflect the two key stereotype dimensions in intergroup perception (Fiske, 2018 ): warmth and competence. These dimensions organize people’s perceptions of social systems: perceived competence reflects groups’ stereotypic status in society. The hierarchy supposedly reflects merit, so rank predicts their supposed competence and evokes respect—or supposed incompetence and disrespect. Besides groups’ status (competence), the other aspect of social structure is groups’ apparent cooperative or competitive goals, interdependencies that stereotypically predict warmth and trustworthiness. Cooperators on our side are nice; competitors are not. Stereotypes derive from social structural perceptions (status and interdependence), especially when people learn about others they might encounter (Fiske, Cuddy, Glick & Xu, 2002 ; Nicolas et al., 2021 ). Black Americans do not get a break on either dimension. And because these racialized perceptions derive from social structure, they pave the way for systemic racism. Consider the evidence for these two dimensions: competence and warmth in racialized perceptions and behavior.

Disrespect communicates Whites’ view of Blacks as low status and incompetent

The default representation of Black Americans is low status (Dupree, Torrez, Obioha & Fiske, 2021 ). Whites spontaneously associate Black faces with low-status jobs, compared to Whites. The structural belief that Blacks are low status appears in associating them with jobs such as janitor, dishwasher, garbage collector, taxi driver, cashier, maid, prostitute. This race–status association correlates with endorsing social dominance (believing that some groups inevitably dominate others, and it is better that way) and with meritocracy (group get what they deserve). All these judgments share a common element of disrespect and assumed incompetence.

Race–status associations emerge in behavior that maintains Black people at the bottom of the hierarchy. Respondents endorsed Black applicants for lower status jobs and withheld support for organizations and government policies aiding minorities. Thus, racialized associations, assumptions, and preferences all identify a view of Black people's structural position as low status, on average. Behavior communicates these attitudes, whether examined or not. Thus, race–status associations imply Black incompetence, covarying with feeling-thermometer (0–100) ratings of interracial bias, social dominance orientation, meritocracy beliefs, as well as hierarchy-maintaining hiring and policy preferences.

Disrespectful behavior that presumes incompetence of Blacks appears in another series of studies. Well-meaning liberals, expected to introduce themselves to a Black partner, dumbed-down their speech, as they did in vocabulary for a task assignment (Dupree & Fiske, 2019 ). Similarly, White Democratic presidential candidates also showed a competence downshift in speeches to minority audiences only (Dupree & Fiske, 2019 ).

This pattern reproduces itself when respondents imagine introducing themselves to a lower-status person (race unspecified) at work (Swencionis & Fiske, 2016 ). They claim their goal is to communicate their own warmth (as they downplay their competence), but this rests on the presumption of the other’s incompetence. Trying to be folksy does not communicate respect.

The presumption that structural status predicts competence is widespread (averaging r > 0.80 across US and international samples; Fiske & Durante, 2016 ). The implication is that for most White Americans, the association that pops into their minds will link a Black person with incompetence. People communicate such disrespect by failing to bet on or invest in the other’s performance (Walsh, Vaida, & Fiske, under review).

Structurally, this amounts to racism. Black people are widely perceived as inferior in these ways, which are baked into the social hierarchy, reflecting disrespectful patterns of interpersonal behavior. All of this perpetuates the social hierarchy and the image of Blacks as incompetent.

Worse yet, disrespect surfaces in police encountering Black drivers. From the first moment (“Hey” instead of “Sir” or “Ma’am”), police officer language shows computationally derived, measurably lower respect (Voigt et al., 2017 ). Given the already fraught relationships between police and Black community members, this worsens an already dangerous encounter and undermines the chances to create trust.

Distrust communicates Whites’ views of Blacks as uncooperative and not warm

Besides incompetence, the other major dimension of social cognition is warmth (trustworthy, friendly), as noted. The default stereotype of a Black person is probably also untrustworthy, but the data on this point are surprisingly indirect. Whites can be expected to distrust Blacks as part of the larger principle that, categorically, people mistrust outgroups. More specifically, as noted, Whites associate Blacks with crime, which certainly undermines trust. 5 This configuration fits survey data showing that ratings of poor (i.e., explicitly low-status) Black people allege incompetence (disrespecting them) but also lack of warmth (distrusting them).

Plotting these ratings in a warmth x competence space, poor Blacks are frequently judged as low on both. Because White Americans link race and status, the low-income Black person is the default Black person, allegedly incompetent, but also untrustworthy. Mistrust is indicated by excessive surveillance of Black Americans (driving while Black, shopping while Black, false accusations of theft or assault, police shootings…). 6

Distrust can be operationalized as behavior: In the economic Trust Game, a player must decide how much of their starting endowment to share, on the knowledge that it will be tripled, and on the hope that their partner will share back, generously. Incentivized trust-game behavior closely tracks warmth ratings; that is, societal groups rated as low warmth and untrustworthy receive less shared endowment, presumably because they are not trusted to share it back. In nationally representative samples, people of color do not fare well in the Trust Game (Walsh et al., under review). In more prosaic settings, non-verbal behavior reveals unmonitored dislike (if not specifically mistrust), as noted.

Black Americans experience repeated treatment as incompetent and untrustworthy. Because this stereotype and ensuing behavior is racially category-based and negative, as well as potentially controllable, it is racist. Because the behavior comes from societal stereotypes, which come from social structure, 7 it is systemic.

Whites’ potential control implies responsibility for reinforcing system racism

Racialized interactions could also be termed racist, in the sense that White people could potentially observe their own inequitable behavior if they chose (Fiske, 1989 ). People rarely examine these unwritten rules, typical behaviors, but conceivably they could, so “unexamined” bias captures the higher potential control for behavior than for implicit associations. Control implies responsibility in the minds of lay people and the law, so this interpretation of “racialized” as “racist” creates concern and is likely to be contested. But the science makes the empirical point here that racialized social behavior is demonstrably controllable, given sufficient incentive (Monteith, Lybarger & Woodcock, 2009 ; Sinclair, Lowery, Hardin & Colangelo, 2005 ). So systematically different behavior by race reflects a racist habit, script, or norm, the components of a system from the bottom up.

The challenge in controlling racist habits is that they are the cultural default. Much of this systematic behavior results from White Americans’ inexperience with Black Americans, thereby substituting societal representations for individuating information about the unique human (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990 ). People use especially those default representations that fit their natural human tendency to detect and prefer people they view as similar to themselves. To unpack this, consider some basic principles of affiliation that would predispose Whites to favor other Whites and exclude Black people. First is the basic tendency to categorize others and to favor those of the ingroup. For decades, principles of attraction have established its foundations in similarity (Byrne, 1971 ; Montoya & Horton, 2013 ) or homophily (McPherson, Smith-Lovin & Cook, 2001 ). And mere categorization suffices to produce ingroup favoritism (Tajfel & Turner, 1979 ). No animus is necessary, although it easily develops. As a basis for categorization, race is arbitrary (more so than gender and age; Kurzban, Tooby & Cosmides, 2001 ) but common (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999 ). Thus, race-based ingroup favoritism is a default, in the absence of other experience. 8 This makes it hard to over-ride.

Societal segregation by race makes difficulties for overcoming the racial default. Segregation limits White exposure to Blacks, undermining their direct experience, leaving Whites to rely on cognitive shortcuts to represent Blacks as a group. Indeed, the less exposure people have to outgroups, the more clearly they differentiate among them–stereotypically. That is, White Americans who know the least about other races have the clearest stereotypes about them; the less diversity, the more differentiated their cognitive representations (Bai, Ramos & Fiske, 2020 ).

What’s wrong with that?

As a scientific question, a skeptic might ask, what’s wrong with differentiating by stereotypes? One set of answers concerns the demeaning individual and face-to-face interaction, just addressed. The other answers pertain to sheer demographic diversity of Black Americans, covered next.

Given its racial history and ongoing systems, societal patterns and cultural stereotypes prevailing in the US tend to associate Blacks with low status and Whites with high status as noted. To the extent this race–status association has a kernel of statistical accuracy (Blacks are over-represented in low-status jobs), it fails several tests as an argument for using stereotypes as a constructive strategy of intergroup relations. First, it ignores variability, individuality, and (especially) Black diversity. Second, category-based thinking exaggerates perceived between-group variability and minimizes perceived within-group variability (Tajfel & Turner 1979 ; Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff & Ruderman, 1978 ). So “nouns that cut slices” (Allport’s, 1954 felicitous phrase for category labels) do violence to the human data. What’s more, society has civil rights laws protecting people from being judged by their group membership, so the consensus is that this is not only wrong, but illegal.

Race–status associations, in practice, ignore all the structural contributors to race–status associations, such as the neighborhood effects, already described. Whites assume meritocracy, believing that status accurately reflects individual competence (Fiske, Dupree, Nicolas & Swencionis, 2016 ); globally, the perceived status—perceived competence correlation hovers around 0.80. (The only countries where people are more cynical about the status-merit link are former Communist ones; Grigoryan et al., 2020 .) The point here is that status has many antecedents, and not all of them are merit (or other personal, stereotypical explanations, e.g., innately good/bad at math). Systemic factors such as neighborhood, school, family resources, connections, and especially race all receive no mention in the meritocracy account.

Whites do differentiate Black Americans by subcategories, e.g., by status, specifically social class, viewing low-income Black people as incompetent and untrustworthy, but Black professionals as competent and trustworthy (Fiske, Cuddy, Glick & Xu, 2002 ). Black Americans themselves differentiate several subtypes of Blacks likewise along a social-class dimension (Fiske, Bergsieker, Russell & Williams, 2009 ).

Status-keeping shortcuts are easier to maintain without information to the contrary, such as experiencing human variability. Whites with less exposure to Blacks are more overtly prejudiced as a function of structural features such as rural residence, where they encounter less diversity (Bai et al., 2020 ), and lack of education, where they experience less variability of ideas. As a structural matter, segregated White rural residence also predicts lower school quality partly because of the American policy of locally funding schools; this creates an association between a weaker tax base, rural location, ethnic homogeneity, and overt bias. These systemic factors interact to produce prejudice. As an earlier section shows, the social structure permeates American arrangements since the arrival of Whites on native lands.

Nevertheless, for most Whites, their isolated lives make them inexperienced about their Black fellow citizens. Housing segregation disfavors most Whites in experience with diversity, making them often inept and naïve when speaking about issues that are facts of Black lives. This means that Whites rely on cultural shortcuts to understand the Black people whose life experience they do not know. These cognitive representations derive from perceived structural patterns such as race–status associations and race-resource unfairness (Krysan & Crowder, 2017 ).

We have seen that Whites’ racial beliefs are relatively automatic (implicit bias) and ambivalent (warmth/competence). The resulting associations (stereotypes) are more subtle than most people believe. They are consequently hard for anyone to detect in themselves (unexamined) or in any one person (under the radar), but the patterns appear systemically as aggregate biases. Supposing the aggregate biases are problematic, at least because they ignore variability, examine that more closely.

Aggregate bias ignores diversity

So far, this review has described the relentless systems of racism that limit opportunity and outcomes by race. Many Black Americans nevertheless succeed despite the rigged system. Black diversity thus results from those who escape the system, but also from African and Caribbean immigration, and from intermarriage. For Black students enrolled at selective colleges, especially, the diversity of their backgrounds is the main fact that underscores their success (Charles, Kramer, Massey & Torres, 2021 ). Any given White student’s background is far more predictable than any given Black student’s, which potentially ranges from extreme disadvantage to extreme wealth. For that minority (a third) of Black students whose segregated neighborhoods entail underfunded schools, gang violence, and concentrated police violence, their presence in college testifies to extraordinary resilience (Charles, Fischer, Mooney & Massey, 2009 ).

Most non-Black people do not realize that Black Americans are more diverse than most American ethnic groups. Underestimating their variety allows an oversimplified image to dominate every level, from mind to society, making it a systemic racism. This section describes diversity based on place, intermarriage, immigrant experience, parent education, and sheer escape.

A century ago, most Black Americans lived in the rural South, but after the Great Migration, most lived in cities, often in the North, usually hyper-segregated, but with family roots in both the North and South. By the turn of the current century, Black American student bodies at selective colleges were the most diverse in history, more biracial, more immigrant, more middle or upper class, and equally identifying themselves as both American and as Black (Charles et al., 2021 ). Black students, even as elites, show “unprecedented variation in terms of racial origins, skin tone, nativity, generation, class, and segregation” (Charles et al., 2021 , Ch. 10).

Clusters of characteristics and attitudes illustrate the variety. Mixed-race students identify less with being Black, are comfortable with both Blacks and Whites, see Whites as less discriminatory, and report deep parental involvement in their schooling and cultural experiences. Mixed race students also have more White friends and fewer Black friends than their monoracial peers and are more likely to date outside the group, especially with Whites. In addition, mixed-race students are less likely to join majority-Black organizations on campus, and thus report less intense interaction with Blacks . Psychologically, the White view of biracial individuals continues to demonstrate hypodescent, i.e., the view that biracial individuals belong to the less advantaged group, or the cognitive expression of the “one drop rule.” Combining the sociological and psychological angle demonstrates the lack of consistency between how biracial Americans are viewed and the way they see themselves.

Black students with an immigrant background are most comfortable with other Black students, and report having strict parents who expect obedience, respect, hard work, and family loyalty without hands-on, hovering involvement. First-generation immigrants, especially African immigrants (versus Caribbean ones), believe in meritocracy and see Whites as not so discriminatory. After a generation, idealism gives way to pragmatism: Hard work pays off. African immigrant origins predict reliably higher grades.

As for segregation, Black students growing up with more exposure to Whites feel closer to them but also view Whites as more discriminatory, a psychologically complex mental state to manage. In contrast, living in segregated neighborhoods especially exposes Black students to higher (the top third) levels of disorder and violence, leading them to view Whites as more distant and discriminatory. But parents are more protective, relying on strict discipline but not trying to use shame or guilt as an influence strategy (more frequent in Asian families).

As with all students, high-school GPA predicts college GPA. Besides that, again as with all students, Black women do better than Black men, as do those with educated parents . Differences in academic preparation vary by segregation in two ways: the more White students in their schools, the worse Black students’ grades but the higher their SATs, suggesting more rigorous standards. Thus, the portraits of Black college students are diverse; generalizations are unreliable, except perhaps for one: resilience in the face of systemic bias and a diversity of adaptations to a variety of challenges.

We document Black diversity here for these reasons: First, to avoid making the litany of systemic Black disadvantages the sole image conveyed here. Second, because of segregation, many White people, including University faculty, see a Black person on campus and—assuming they realize this is a student—they presume the person comes from a low-income background, unprepared for college, with uneducated parents, native born, but with little experience outside the imagined ghetto, etc. This may be true for some small fraction of students, but not just the Black ones, and not true of most Black students on campus today. A third reason to remind the reader of Black diversity on campus is to highlight experiences of inter-racial contact as important one mechanism for overcoming racial bias, and—if scaled up to integrated neighborhoods, schools, and workplaces—for shifting systemic racism.

Contact: exposure to racial diversity

People with least exposure to diversity have the most differentiated images of the outgroups they have never met (Bai et al., 2020 ). And the prospect and first experience of diversity is not salutary; newly diverse contexts show lower well-being (Putnam, 2007 ; Ramos, Bennett, Massey & Hewstone, 2019 ). But over time, people get used to each other: well-being is higher and stereotypes melt into each, forming an undifferentiated cluster of people like us, mostly warm and competent.

Psychology has 70 years of research to explain how this works, following Allport’s ( 1954 ) contact hypothesis. In one meta-analytic perspective (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006 ), intergroup contact reduces prejudice, the more it meets Allport’s conditions: shared goals, non-trivial interactions, authority sanctions, and rewarding results. Much of the process seems to be affect-driven. If the contact setting would afford the opportunity for friendship, the contact effect is stronger (Tropp & Pettigrew, 2005 ). This is a useful reminder that much prejudice is emotional, not cognitive. In fact, a meta-analysis of 50 years of research on racist attitudes found that they predict racist behavior the most when they are emotions (“hating them”) rather than stereotypes (“they are lazy”) or even simple evaluations (2 on a 5-point scale) (Talaska et al., 2008 ).

Nevertheless, the core element of successful contact, goal interdependence, does operate via information processing. In laboratory experiments, interdependence makes people attend specifically to unexpected, stereotype-inconsistent information, and they make dispositional inferences, generating an individualized coherent impression of the teammate (Ames & Fiske, 2013 ; Erber & Fiske, 1984 ). Neural signatures of mindreading prominently include the mPFC regions that reliably activate when people are inferring another’s predispositions. The mind-reading mPFC activates most for an interdependent partner’s stereotype-inconsistent attributes. Although supporting evidence includes these mechanisms, a subsequent meta-analysis (Paluck, Porat, Clark & Green, 2021 ) notes that few high-quality intergroup studies have focused on race per se, few look at adults, few are experiments. We have much to learn.

Conclusion: systemic racism is individual/interpersonal and institutional/societal but rarely recognized

Segregated housing disadvantages many Black Americans, and its effects are far-reaching, not only in life opportunities and outcomes (education, employment, health, well-being) but also in the psychology of systemic racism. We have argued that case here. Most Whites fail to recognize and appreciate the growing diversity of America’s Black population, which has arisen from a mixture of Black resilience, a growing middle class, rising intermarriage, and global-South immigration. Generally, White Americans—because of the segregation perpetuated to sustain their advantage—have limited exposure to Black Americans, so their knowledge is indirect, and based on cultural caricatures. Segregation allows White people to be clueless about race, and because racial bias is more automatic, ambiguous, and ambivalent than people think, they fail to detect it in themselves and others. As a result, White people have many unexamined biases, undergirded by earlier stages of information processing (e.g., attention, perception, learning, memory, reasoning) that sustain such a lack of awareness. These cognitive errors and biases stem from lack of exposure, lack of the accurate evidence, and a lack of necessary knowledge.

The assumption here is that if people were simply made aware of the facts that have been described in the earlier sections, they would slap their palm to their head and immediately vote for reparations. But as readers may no doubt deduce on their own, confronting accurate data and internalizing it is not a smooth or pretty process. That our minds resist information that challenges certain types of prior beliefs is a fundamental discovery from the mind sciences. Basic cognitive processes such as motivated cognition help to maintain a lack of awareness of racial experiences as they exist on the ground. But no lack of awareness need exist.

The human ability for conscious awareness, deliberate thought, and the motivation to link values to behavior cannot be underestimated as vehicles of change. We have accomplished this regarding how we understand the relationship of Earth to our Sun, so we know it is not as it seems. If we choose, we can similarly put our minds to derive the best evidence to learn about the presence or absence of systemic racism. If we can acquire the appropriate knowledge (often hidden from our conscious perception), we will be more likely to remain open to evidence that shows its presence.

If we do not undertake this effort, it is at our own peril. If, in the twenty-first century, we cannot mount a new struggle to see the social world for what it is, we are by choice dooming ourselves to extended ignorance that will be costly to us, our society, and the world we inevitably leave to our descendants. Earlier we provided evidence about unexpected (by scientists) decreases in implicit sexuality bias (massive drop) and race bias (more modest change) since 2007. These data provide optimism that mental content that we cannot change at will is nonetheless capable of movement toward racial neutrality across the US.

In other words, who-we-have-been need not be the future-selves-we-are-becoming. Here, we demonstrated that grappling with the correct data is a necessary step on the path to understanding our role in the creation of systemic racism. Among the blind spots that we will need to shake off, once and for all, is the belief that racism is the product of a few bad people in our society, and that removing them from power will suffice to deal with the issue.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the constructive comments of James Jones, Jeremey Wolfe, and Reviewer 1.

Author contributions

All authors planned, wrote, read, and approved the final manuscript.

No funding and no major differences in the active participation in all phases of planning and producing this paper.

Declarations

Because this is a review of published work, this paper is not itself new human subjects research, so it required no new ethics approval, no consent to participate, and no new data to share.

The authors do consent to publish this paper in this journal.

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

1 Space and time preclude our covering the targets’ perspective, identity, resilience. Nor do we cover racial socialization in children.

2 Through the sensory and perceptual systems granted to our species by evolution, these dyadic and small-group social interactions evolve into larger and larger social units, such as the hundreds of so-called friends or millions of so-called followers on more recent forms of social media. Today we transcend ancestral, small-group interactions to generate larger-scale groups whose interactions occur on an exponential scale. The internet provides avenues for the high-speed transmission of individual attitudes, beliefs, values, as well as for propelling action across communities and nations. These communications have the potential to spread both social good and social harm, with explicit racial animus and implicit prejudicial bias being examples of the latter.

3 Using the most common measure of segregation (the dissimilarity index), in that year 94% Black metropolitan residents lived under conditions of “high” segregation (an index of 60 or greater on a 0–100 scale), meaning that at least 60% of Blacks would have to exchange neighborhoods with Whites to achieve an even distribution of the races across neighborhoods (Rugh & Massey, 2014 ). Moreover, in a subset of metropolitan areas, not only were Black residents unevenly distributed across neighborhoods, they were also isolated within overwhelmingly Black districts that were themselves densely clustered near the central business district, a geographic pattern that Massey and Denton ( 1989 ) labeled "hypersegregation.”

4 The NLP fits more traditional findings, a form of cross-validation. Based on content analysis of an 84-adjective checklist, the language describing Black Americans did not change much, across samples from 1933 to 2007 (Bergsieker, Leslie, Constantine, & Fiske, 2012 , Study 4): The most recent data describe ambivalent view of sociality (aggressive, gregarious, passionate), and some specific stereotypes (loud, talkative, religious, loyal to family, sportsmanlike, musical, materialistic), but saying nothing about competence. Neglecting to mention an obvious dimension can reveal taboo topics, stereotyping by omission (Bergsieker et al., 2012 ).

5 Black people may distrust Whites, too, but they have less standing (status and power) to do damage.

6 An odd anomaly: Abundant research describes Black people’s generalized trust as lower then Whites’ generalized trust. Also, social science has studied Black Americans’ mistrust of government, business, healthcare, and education systems that have historically abused them (see next section). This would hardly seem puzzling enough to be the lion’s share of the trust literature and to eclipse White Americans’ pockets of mistrust. Specifically, no one seems to study Whites’ mistrust of Black people. Overlooking the obvious is one symptom of a systemic bias.

7 The combination of status-competence and warmth-trustworthiness creates remarkably stable perceptions of social structure (Durante et al., 2015). In social systems across the globe, middle classes are stereotypically competent and warm (trustworthy) whereas homeless people are neither. And in the mixed quadrants, rich people seem competent but cold, whereas old people seem well-intentioned but incompetent. These class and age patterns are nearly universal. In contrast, ethnic, racial, religious, and other cultural stereotypes are accidents of history, reflecting what subset of a group arrived under what circumstances. Compare stereotypes of Chinese railroad workers in the nineteenth century to stereotypes of Chinese entrepreneurs in the twenty-first century.

8 Implicit bias is difficult to monitor, as noted. Yet another way that prejudice goes undetected, is in its modern form, of being exhibited less as outgroup harm and instead as ingroup help (Greenwald & Pettigrew, 2014 ). Despite this ambiguity, the net effect is the same—just harder to detect, and even lauded, because helping is a prosocial act that garners praise.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

  • Allport G. The nature of prejudice. Addison-Wesley; 1954. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ames DL, Fiske ST. Outcome dependency alters the neural substrates of impression formation. NeuroImage. 2013; 83 :599–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.001. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bai X, Ramos MR, Fiske ST. As diversity increases, people paradoxically perceive social groups as more similar. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2020; 117 (23):12741–12749. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Banaji MR, Greenwald AG. Blindspot: Hidden biases of good people. Random House; 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bauman JF. Public housing, race, and renewal: Urban planning in Philadelphia, 1920–1974. Temple University Press; 1987. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bergsieker HB, Leslie LM, Constantine VS, Fiske ST. Stereotyping by omission: Eliminate the negative, accentuate the positive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2012; 102 (6):1214–1238. doi: 10.1037/a0027717. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bertrand M, Mullainathan S. Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. American Economic Review. 2004; 94 (4):991–1013. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bickford A, Massey DS. Segregation in the second ghetto: Racial and ethnic segregation in American public housing, 1977. Social Forces. 1991; 69 :1011–1038. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blair IV, Judd CM, Chapleau KM. The influence of Afrocentric facial features in criminal sentencing. Psychological Science. 2004; 15 :674–679. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bobo LD, Charles CZ, Krysan M, Simmons AD. The real record on racial attitudes. In: Marsden PV, editor. Social trends in the United States: Evidence from the General Social Survey since, 1972. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 2012. pp. 38–93. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Byrne D. The attraction paradigm. Academic Press; 1971. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carpusor AG, Loges WE. Rental discrimination and ethnicity in names. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2006; 36 (4):934–952. doi: 10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00050.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charles CZ. “The dynamics of racial residential segregation. Annual Review of Sociology. 2003; 29 :167–207. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charles CZ, Fischer MJ, Mooney MA, Massey DS. Taming the river: Negotiating the academic, financial, and social currents in America’s Selective Colleges and Universities. Princeton University Press; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charles CZ, Kramer RA, Massey DS, Torres KC. Divergent currents: The diverse origins of the new black elite. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 2021. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charlesworth TES, Banaji MR. Patterns of implicit and explicit stereotypes III: Long-term change in gender stereotypes. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2021 doi: 10.1177/1948550620988425. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charlesworth, T.E.S., & Banaji, M. R. (in press). The relationship of implicit social cognition and discriminatory behavior. In A. Deshpande (Ed.) Handbook on the Economics of Discrimination and Affirmative Action . Edward Elgar.
  • Chetty R, Hendren N, Jones MR, Porter SR. Race and economic opportunity in the United States: An intergenerational perspective. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2020; 135 (2):711–783. doi: 10.1093/qje/qjz042. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Correll J, Wittenbrink B, Park B, Judd CM, Goyle A. Dangerous enough: Moderating racial bias with contextual threat cues. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2010; 47 :184–189. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.08.017. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Craig MA, Richeson JA. More diverse yet less tolerant? How the increasingly diverse racial landscape affects Americans’ racial attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2014; 40 (6):750–761. doi: 10.1177/0146167214524993. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Devine PG. Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1989; 56 (1):5–18. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dickinson Jr., W. B. (1963). Urban renewal under fire. Editorial Research Reports , 1963 (Vol. II). Available at http://library.cqpress.com/cqresearcher/cqresrre1963082100
  • Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL, editors. Prejudice, discrimination, and racism. Academic Press; 1986. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002; 82 (1):62–68. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DuBois WEB. Worlds of color. Foreign Affairs. 1925; 3 (3):423–444. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duncan OD. Inheritance of poverty or inheritance of race? In: Moynihan DP, editor. On understanding poverty: Perspectives from the social sciences. Basic Books; 1969. pp. 85–110. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dupree CH, Fiske ST. Self-presentation in interracial settings: The competence downshift by white liberals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2019; 117 (3):579–604. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000166. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dupree CH, Torrez B, Obioha OA, Fiske ST. Race-status associations: Distinct effects of three novel measures among white and black perceivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2021; 120 (3):601–625. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000257. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eberhardt JL. Biased: Uncovering the hidden prejudice that shapes what we see, think, and do. New York: Viking; 2019. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Emerson MO, Chai KJ, Yancey G. Does race matter in residential segregation? Exploring the preferences of white Americans. American Sociological Review. 2001; 66 (6):922–935. doi: 10.2307/3088879. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Erber R, Fiske ST. Outcome dependency and attention to inconsistent information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1984; 47 :709–726. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fairlie, H. 1985. The art of revival: Washington diarist-ex-presidents. The New Republic , May 6.
  • Farley R, Allen WR. The color line and the quality of life in America. Russell Sage Foundation; 1987. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feagin JR. Excluding blacks and others from housing: The foundation of white racism. Cityscape. 1999; 4 (3):79–91. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feagin JR. Systemic racism: A theory of oppression. Routledge; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske ST. Examining the role of intent: Toward understanding its role in stereotyping and prejudice. In: Uleman J, Bargh J, editors. Unintended thought: The limits of awareness, intention, and control. Guilford; 1989. pp. 253–283. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske ST. Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist. 1993; 48 :621–628. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske, S. T. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In The handbook of social psychology, Vols. 1–2, 4th ed (pp. 357–411). McGraw-Hill.
  • Fiske ST. Stereotype content: Warmth and competence endure. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2018; 27 (2):67–73. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske ST, Bergsieker H, Russell AM, Williams L. Images of Black Americans: Then, “them” and now, “Obama!” DuBois Review: Social Science Research on Race. 2009; 6 :83–101. doi: 10.1017/S1742058X0909002X. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske ST, Cuddy AJ, Glick P, Xu J. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002; 82 :878–902. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske ST, Dupree CH, Nicolas G, Swencionis JK. Status, power, and intergroup relations: The personal is the societal. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2016; 11 :44–48. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.012. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske ST, Durante F. Stereotype content across cultures: Variations on a few themes. In: Gelfand MJ, Chiu C-Y, Hong Y-Y, editors. Handbook of advances in culture and psychology. Oxford University Press; 2016. pp. 209–258. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske ST, Neuberg SL. A continuum model of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influence of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Academic Press; 1990. pp. 1–74. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fiske ST, Taylor SE. Social cognition: From brains to culture (4/e) London: Sage; 2021. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Foner E. A short history of reconstruction, 1863–1877. Harper & Row; 1990. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goff PA, Eberhardt JL, Williams MJ, Jackson MC. Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2008; 94 (2):292–306. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.292. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review. 1995; 102 (1):4–27. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenwald AG, Pettigrew TF. With malice toward none and charity for some: Ingroup favoritism enables discrimination. American Psychologist. 2014; 69 (7):669–684. doi: 10.1037/a0036056. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenwald, A. G., Brendl, M., Cai, H., Cvencek, D., Dovidio, J., Friese, M., Hahn, A., Hehman, E., Hofmann, W., Hughes, S., Hussey, I., Jordan, C. H., Jost, J., Kirby, T. A., Lai, C. K., Lang, J. W. B., Lindgren, K. P., Maison, D., Ostafin, B., Rae, J. R., ... Wiers, R. (2020). The implicit association test at age 20: What is known and what is not known about implicit bias. https://psyarxiv.com/bf97c .
  • Grigoryan L, Bai X, Durante F, Fiske ST, Berdyna EM, Fabrykant M, Hakobjanyan KA, Kotova M, Makashvili A, Morozova-Larina O, Mullabaeva N, Samekin A, Verbilovich V, Yahiiaiev I. Stereotypes as Historical Accidents: Images of Social Class Stereotypes in Postcommunist Versus Capitalist Societies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2020; 46 (6):927–943. doi: 10.1177/0146167219881434. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guthrie, R. V. (2004). Even the rat was white: A historical view of psychology (Classic ed., 2nd ed). Allyn and Bacon.
  • Hanson A, Hawley Z. Do landlords discriminate in the rental housing market? Evidence from an internet field experiment in US Cities. Journal of Urban Economics. 2011; 70 (2):99–114. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Helper R. Racial policies and practices of real estate brokers. University of Minnesota Press; 1969. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higginbotham AL. Shades of freedom: Racial politics and presumptions of the American legal process. Oxford University Press; 1998. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hirsch AR. Making the second ghetto: Race and housing in Chicago, 1940–1960. Cambridge University Press; 1983. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Intrator J, Tannen J, Massey DS. Segregation by Race and Income in the United States, 1970–2010. Social Science Research. 2016; 60 (1):45–60. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2016.08.003. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jackson KT. Crabgrass frontier: The suburbanization of the United States. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1985. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jones JM. The political dimensions of black liberation. The Black Scholar. 1971; 3 (1):67–75. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jones JM. Prejudice and racism. Addison-Wesley; 1972. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jones, J. M. (1997). Prejudice and racism (2nd ed). McGraw-Hill Companies.
  • Jost JT. A theory of system justification. Harvard University Press; 2020. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jost JT, Banaji MR. The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology. 1994; 33 (1):1–27. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979; 47 (4):263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Katznelson I. When affirmative action was white: An untold history of racial inequality in twentieth-century America. W. W. Norton; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kay AC, Gaucher D, Peach JM, Laurin K, Friesen J, Zanna MP, Spencer SJ. Inequality, discrimination, and the power of the status quo: Direct evidence for a motivation to see the way things are as the way they should be. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009; 97 (3):421–434. doi: 10.1037/a0015997. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krysan M, Couper MP, Farley R, Forman T. Does race matter in neighborhood preferences? Results from a video experiment. AJS; American Journal of Sociology. 2009; 115 (2):527–559. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krysan M, Crowder K. Cycle of segregation: Social processes and residential stratification. Russell Sage Foundation; 2017. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kunda Z. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin. 1990; 108 (3):480–498. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kurdi B, Carroll TJ, Banaji MR. Specificity and incremental predictive validity of implicit attitudes: Studies of a race-based phenotype. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications. 2021; 6 :61. doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00324-y. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kurdi B, Seitchik AE, Axt JR, Carroll TJ, Karapetyan A, Kaushik N, Tomezsko D, Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Relationship between the implicit association test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist. 2019; 74 (5):569–586. doi: 10.1037/amp0000364. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kurzban R, Tooby J, Cosmides L. Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2001; 98 (26):15387–15392. doi: 10.1073/pnas.251541498. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lemann N. The promised land: The great black migration and how it changed America. Knopf; 1991. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levin DT, Banaji MR. Distortions in the perceived lightness of faces: The role of race categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2006; 135 (4):501–512. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Loewen JW. Sundown towns: A hidden dimension of American racism. New Press; 2018. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Madigan T. The burning: Massacre, destruction, and the Tulsa race riot of, 1921. Martin’s Press; 2001. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS. Inheritance of poverty or inheritance of place? The emerging consensus on neighborhoods and stratification. Contemporary Sociology. 2013; 42 :690–697. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS. Residential segregation is the linchpin of racial stratification. City and Community. 2016; 15 (1):4–7. doi: 10.1111/cico.12145. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS. Segregation in 21st century America. Journal of Catholic Social Thought. 2018; 15 (2):235–260. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS. Still the linchpin: Segregation and stratification in the USA. Race and Social Problems. 2020; 12 (1):1–12. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Brodmann S. Spheres of influence: The social ecology of racial and class inequality. Russell Sage Foundation; 2014. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Denton NA. Spatial assimilation as a socioeconomic outcome. American Sociological Review. 1985; 50 (1):94–105. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Denton NA. Hypersegregation in US metropolitan areas: Black and Hispanic segregation along five dimensions. Demography. 1989; 26 (3):373–393. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Denton NA. American apartheid: Segregation and the making of the underclass. Harvard University Press; 1993. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Fischer MJ. The ecology of racial discrimination. City and Community. 2004; 3 (3):221–243. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Kanaiaupuni SM. Public housing and the concentration of poverty. Social Science Quarterly. 1993; 74 (1):109–123. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Lundy G. Use of black English and racial discrimination in urban housing markets: New methods and findings. Urban Affairs Review. 2001; 36 (4):470–96. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Tannen J. A research note on trends in black hypersegregation. Demography. 2015; 52 (3):1025–1034. doi: 10.1007/s13524-015-0381-6. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Tannen J. Suburbanization and segregation in the United States: 1970–2010. Ethnic and Racial Studies. 2017; 41 (9):1594–1611. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey DS, Tannen J. Segregation, race, and the social worlds of rich and poor. In: Braun H, Kirsch I, editors. The dynamics of opportunity in America: Evidence and perspectives. Springer; 2016. pp. 13–33. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massey, D. S., & Rugh, J. S. (2018). The intersection of race and class: Zoning, affordable housing, and segregation in U.S. Metropolitan Areas. In: Gregory Squires (Ed.), The fight for fair housing: Causes, consequences and future implications of the 1968 federal fair housing act (pp. 245–265). Taylor and Francis.
  • McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM. Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology. 2001; 27 (1):415–444. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.415. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mead GH. Mind, self, and society: from the standpoint of a social behaviorist. University of Chicago Press; 1934. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Metcalf GR. Fair housing comes of age. Greenwood Press; 1988. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moberg SP, Krysan M, Christianson D. Racial attitudes in America. Public Opinion Quarterly. 2019; 83 (2):450–471. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfz014. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Monteith MJ, Lybarger JE, Woodcock A. Schooling the cognitive monster: the role of motivation in the regulation and control of prejudice. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2009; 3 (3):211–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00177.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Montoya RM, Horton RS. A meta-analytic investigation of the processes underlying the similarity-attraction effect. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2013; 30 (1):64–94. doi: 10.1177/0265407512452989. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morehouse, K., Maddox, K., & Banaji, M. (2021). All human groups are human, but some groups are more human than others. In: Poster presentation at the APS annual convention conference .
  • Nicolas G, Fiske ST, Koch A, Imhoff R, Unkelbach C, Terache J, Carrier A, Yzerbyt V. Relational versus structural goals prioritize different social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2021 doi: 10.1037/pspi0000366. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Norton MI, Sommers SR. Whites see racism as a zero-sum game that they are now losing. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011; 6 (3):215–218. doi: 10.1177/1745691611406922. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orren K. Corporate power and social change: The politics of the life insurance industry. The Johns Hopkins University Press; 1974. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Owens A. Unequal opportunity: School and neighborhood segregation in the USA. Race and Social Problems. 2020; 12 (1):29–41. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Packard JM. American nightmare: The history of Jim Crow. Martin’s Press; 2003. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pager D. The mark of a criminal record. American Journal of Sociology. 2003; 108 (5):937–975. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pager D, Western B, Bonikowski B. Discrimination in a low-wage labor market: A field experiment. American Sociological Review. 2009; 74 (5):777–799. doi: 10.1177/000312240907400505. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paluck EL, Porat R, Clark CS, Green DP. Prejudice reduction: Progress and challenges. Annual Review of Psychology. 2021; 72 (1):533–560. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-071620-030619. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Parker CS. Status threat: Moving the right further to the right? Daedalus. 2021; 150 (2):56–75. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Payne BK, Vuletich HA, Brown-Iannuzzi JL. Historical roots of implicit bias in slavery. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2019; 116 :11693–11698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1818816116. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Payne BK, Vuletich HA, Lundberg KB. The bias of crowds: How implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychological Inquiry. 2017; 28 :233–248. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2017.1335568. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peterson RD, Krivo LJ. Divergent Social Worlds: Neighborhood Crime and the Racial-Spatial Divide. Russell Sage Foundation; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pettigrew T. Racial change and social policy. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 1979; 441 :114–131. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pettigrew TF, Tropp LR. A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006; 90 (5):751–783. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Philpott T. The slum and the ghetto: Neighborhood deterioration and middle class reform, Chicago 1880–1930. Oxford University Press; 1978. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Putnam R. E Pluribus Unum: Diversity and community in the twenty-first century: The 2006 Johan Skytte prize lecture. Scandinavian Political Studies. 2007; 30 (2):137–174. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9477.2007.00176.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Quillian L, Lee JJ, Honoré B. Racial discrimination in the U.S. housing and mortgage lending markets: a quantitative review of trends, 1976–2016. Race and Social Problems. 2020; 12 (1):13–28. doi: 10.1007/s12552-019-09276-x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Quillian L, Pager D. Black neighbors, higher crime? The role of racial stereotypes in evaluations of neighborhood crime. American Journal of Sociology. 2001; 107 (3):717–767. doi: 10.1086/338938. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Quillian L, Pager D. Estimating risk: Stereotype Amplification and the perceived risk of criminal victimization. Social Psychology Quarterly. 2010; 73 (1):79–104. doi: 10.1177/0190272509360763. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ramos MR, Bennett MR, Massey DS, Hewstone M. Humans adapt to social diversity over time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2019; 116 (25):12244–12249. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1818884116. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Richeson JA, Shelton JN. Negotiating interracial interactions: Costs, consequences, and possibilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007; 16 (6):316–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00528.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ross SL, Yinger J. The color of credit: Mortgage discrimination, research methodology, and fair-lending enforcement. MIT Press; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rothstein R. The color of law: A forgotten history of how our government segregated America. Liveright; 2017. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rothwell J, Massey DS. The effect of density zoning on racial segregation in U.S. Urban areas. Urban Affairs Review. 2009; 44 (6):779–806. doi: 10.1177/1078087409334163. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rothwell JT, Massey DS. Density zoning and class segregation in U.S. metropolitan areas. Social Science Quarterly. 2010; 91 (5):1123–1143. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rugh JS, Massey DS. Segregation in post-civil rights America: Stalled integration or end of the segregated century? The DuBois Review. 2014; 11 (2):202–232. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salter PS, Adams G, Perez MJ. Racism in the structure of everyday worlds: A cultural-psychological perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2018; 27 (3):150–155. doi: 10.1177/0963721417724239. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sampson RJ. Great American City: Chicago and the enduring neighborhood effect. University of Chicago Press; 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Satter B. Family properties: How the struggle over race and real estate transformed Chicago and Urban America. Picador; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sharkey P. Stuck in Place: Urban neighborhoods and the end of progress toward racial equality. University of Chicago Press; 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schuman H, Steeh C, Bobo LD, Krysan M. Racial attitudes in America: Trends and interpretations. Revised. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1998. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sidanius J, Pratto F. Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and Oppression. Cambridge University Press; 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sinclair S, Lowery BS, Hardin CD, Colangelo A. Social tuning of automatic racial attitudes: The role of affiliative motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005; 89 (4):583–592. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.583. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stanley DA, Sokol-Hessner P, Banaji MR, Phelps EA. Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011; 108 (19):7710–7715. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014345108. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Staples, B. (2018, June 17). Opinion | The racist trope that won’t die. The New York Times . https://www.nytimes.com/2018/06/17/opinion/roseanne-racism-blacks-apes.html .
  • Sugrue T. Sweet land of liberty: The forgotten struggle for civil rights in the north. Random House; 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swencionis JK, Fiske ST. Promote up, ingratiate down: Status comparisons drive warmth-competence tradeoffs in impression management. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2016; 64 :27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.01.004. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tajfel H, Turner JC. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: Austin WG, Worchel S, editors. The social psychology of inter-group relations. Brooks/Cole; 1979. pp. 33–47. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Talaska CA, Fiske ST, Chaiken S. Legitimating racial discrimination: A meta-analysis of the racial attitude-behavior literature shows that emotions, not beliefs, best predict discrimination. Social Justice Research: Social Power in Action. 2008; 21 :263–296. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor SE, Fiske ST, Etcoff NL, Ruderman AJ. Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1978; 36 :778–793. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.7.778. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tolnay SE, Beck EM. A festival of violence: An analysis of southern lynchings, 1882–1930. University of Illinois Press; 1995. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tropp LR, Pettigrew TF. Differential relationships between intergroup contact and affective and cognitive indicators of prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2005; 31 :1145–1158. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tuttle WM. Race riot: Chicago in the red summer of 1919. Atheneum; 1970. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Voigt R, Camp NP, Prabhakaran V, Hamilton WL, Hetey RC, Griffiths CM, et al. Language from police body camera footage shows racial disparities in officer respect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2017; 114 (25):6521–6526. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waldstreicher D. Slavery's Constitution: From Revolution to Ratification. Hill and Wang; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wallace DF (2009) This is water: Some thoughts, delivered on a significant occasion, about living a compassionate life . Little, Brown and Company. (2005 Kenyon College Commencement address). https://fs.blog/david-foster-wallace-this-is-water/ .
  • Wilkerson I. The warmth of other suns: The epic story of America's great migration. Random House; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Williams JC. White working class: Overcoming class cluelessness in America. Harvard Business Press; 2019. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Word CO, Zanna MP, Cooper J. The nonverbal mediation of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1974; 10 (2):109–120. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(74)90059-6. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zárate MA, Reyna C, Alvarez MJ. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Academic Press; 2019. Cultural inertia, identity, and intergroup dynamics in a changing context; pp. 175–233. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Foundations for Being Alive Now
  • On Being with Krista Tippett
  • Poetry Unbound
  • Subscribe to The Pause Newsletter
  • What Is The On Being Project?
  • Support On Being

Albert Einstein’s Essay on Racial Bias in 1946

Trent T. Gilliss

July 12, 2016

In the years after World War II, Albert Einstein took up the mantle of confronting racism in America. He became a good friend and comrade of the prominent opera singer Paul Robeson, co-chaired an anti-lynching campaign, and was an outspoken supporter of W.E.B. Du Bois . But, it was in January 1946, that he penned one of his most articulate and eloquent essays advocating for the civil rights of black people in America . And, as described in Einstein on Race and Racism , the iconic physicist equated the ghettoization of Jews in Germany and segregation in America, calling racism America’s “worst disease.” Originally published in the January 1946 issue of Pageant magazine, Albert Einstein’s essay was intended to address a primarily white readership:

The Negro Question by Albert Einstein I am writing as one who has lived among you in America only a little more than ten years, and I am writing seriously and warningly. Many readers may ask: “What right has he to speak about things which concern us alone, and which no newcomer should touch?” I do not think such a standpoint is justified. One who has grown up in an environment takes much for granted. On the other hand, one who has come to this country as a mature person may have a keen eye for everything peculiar and characteristic. I believe he should speak out freely on what he sees and feels, for by so doing he may perhaps prove himself useful. What soon makes the new arrival devoted to this country is the democratic trait among the people. I am not thinking here so much of the democratic political constitution of this country, however highly it must be praised. I am thinking of the relationship between individual people and of the attitude they maintain toward one another. In the United States everyone feels assured of his worth as an individual. No one humbles himself before another person or class. Even the great difference in wealth, the superior power of a few, cannot undermine this healthy self-confidence and natural respect for the dignity of one’s fellow-man. There is, however, a somber point in the social outlook of Americans. Their sense of equality and human dignity is mainly limited to men of white skins. Even among these there are prejudices of which I as a Jew am clearly conscious; but they are unimportant in comparison with the attitude of the “Whites” toward their fellow-citizens of darker complexion, particularly toward Negroes. The more I feel an American, the more this situation pains me. I can escape the feeling of complicity in it only by speaking out. Many a sincere person will answer: “Our attitude towards Negroes is the result of unfavorable experiences which we have had by living side by side with Negroes in this country. They are not our equals in intelligence, sense of responsibility, reliability.” I am firmly convinced that whoever believes this suffers from a fatal misconception. Your ancestors dragged these black people from their homes by force; and in the white man’s quest for wealth and an easy life they have been ruthlessly suppressed and exploited, degraded into slavery. The modern prejudice against Negroes is the result of the desire to maintain this unworthy condition. The ancient Greeks also had slaves. They were not Negroes but white men who had been taken captive in war. There could be no talk of racial differences. And yet Aristotle, one of the great Greek philosophers, declared slaves inferior beings who were justly subdued and deprived of their liberty. It is clear that he was enmeshed in a traditional prejudice from which, despite his extraordinary intellect, he could not free himself. A large part of our attitude toward things is conditioned by opinions and emotions which we unconsciously absorb as children from our environment. In other words, it is tradition — besides inherited aptitudes and qualities — which makes us what we are. We but rarely reflect how relatively small as compared with the powerful influence of tradition is the influence of our conscious thought upon our conduct and convictions. It would be foolish to despise tradition. But with our growing self-consciousness and increasing intelligence we must begin to control tradition and assume a critical attitude toward it, if human relations are ever to change for the better. We must try to recognize what in our accepted tradition is damaging to our fate and dignity — and shape our lives accordingly. I believe that whoever tries to think things through honestly will soon recognize how unworthy and even fatal is the traditional bias against Negroes. What, however, can the man of good will do to combat this deeply rooted prejudice? He must have the courage to set an example by word and deed, and must watch lest his children become influenced by this racial bias. I do not believe there is a way in which this deeply entrenched evil can be quickly healed. But until this goal is reached there is no greater satisfaction for a just and well-meaning person than the knowledge that he has devoted his best energies to the service of the good cause.

Contributors

Trent T. Gilliss's photo.

Trent T. Gilliss was the founding executive editor of On Being Studios. He joined Speaking of Faith at American Public Media in 2003 as a web producer and later online editor, and became a co-founder of On Being ‘s move to independence in 2013 (as Krista Tippett Public Productions). He created  On Being’ s original award-winning digital presence, helped envision and oversee the design and buildout of the Loring Park studio space, and curated On Being ‘s first weekly email newsletter. He also served as chief content officer and publisher between 2013 and 2017.

Share your reflection

You may also like.

essay on racial bias

June 11, 2020

Talking About Whiteness

You can’t think about something if you can’t talk about it, says Eula Biss. The writer helpfully opens up lived words and ideas like complacence, guilt, and opportunity hoarding for an urgent reckoning with whiteness. This conversation was inspired by her 2015 essay in The New York Times , “ White Debt .”  

Search results for “ ”

  • Standard View
  • Becoming Wise
  • Creating Our Own Lives
  • This Movie Changed Me
  • Wisdom Practice

On Being Studios

  • Live Events
  • Poetry Films

Lab for the Art of Living

  • Poetry at On Being
  • Cogenerational Social Healing
  • Starting Points & Care Packages
  • Better Conversations Guide
  • Grounding Virtues

Gatherings & Quiet Conversations

  • Social Healing Fellowship
  • Krista Tippett
  • Lucas Johnson
  • Work with Us
  • On Dakota Land

Follow On Being

  • The On Being Project is located on Dakota land.
  • Art at On Being
  • Our 501(c)(3)
  • Privacy Policy
  • Terms of Use

Numbers, Facts and Trends Shaping Your World

Read our research on:

Full Topic List

Regions & Countries

  • Publications
  • Our Methods
  • Short Reads
  • Tools & Resources

Read Our Research On:

  • Black Americans Have a Clear Vision for Reducing Racism but Little Hope It Will Happen

Many say key U.S. institutions should be rebuilt to ensure fair treatment

Table of contents.

  • Black Americans see little improvement in their lives despite increased national attention to racial issues
  • Few Black adults expect equality for Black people in the U.S.
  • Black adults say racism and police brutality are extremely big problems for Black people in the U.S.
  • Personal experiences with discrimination are widespread among Black Americans
  • 2. Black Americans’ views on political strategies, leadership and allyship for achieving equality
  • The legacy of slavery affects Black Americans today
  • Most Black adults agree the descendants of enslaved people should be repaid
  • The types of repayment Black adults think would be most helpful
  • Responsibility for reparations and the likelihood repayment will occur
  • Black adults say the criminal justice system needs to be completely rebuilt
  • Black adults say political, economic and health care systems need major changes to ensure fair treatment
  • Most Black adults say funding for police departments should stay the same or increase
  • Acknowledgments
  • Appendix: Supplemental tables
  • The American Trends Panel survey methodology

Photo showing visitors at the Martin Luther King Jr. Memorial in Washington, D.C. (Astrid Riecken/picture alliance via Getty Images)

Pew Research Center conducted this analysis to understand the nuances among Black people on issues of racial inequality and social change in the United States. This in-depth survey explores differences among Black Americans in their views on the social status of the Black population in the U.S.; their assessments of racial inequality; their visions for institutional and social change; and their outlook on the chances that these improvements will be made. The analysis is the latest in the Center’s series of in-depth surveys of public opinion among Black Americans (read the first, “ Faith Among Black Americans ” and “ Race Is Central to Identity for Black Americans and Affects How They Connect With Each Other ”).

The online survey of 3,912 Black U.S. adults was conducted Oct. 4-17, 2021. Black U.S. adults include those who are single-race, non-Hispanic Black Americans; multiracial non-Hispanic Black Americans; and adults who indicate they are Black and Hispanic. The survey includes 1,025 Black adults on Pew Research Center’s American Trends Panel (ATP) and 2,887 Black adults on Ipsos’ KnowledgePanel. Respondents on both panels are recruited through national, random sampling of residential addresses.

Recruiting panelists by phone or mail ensures that nearly all U.S. Black adults have a chance of selection. This gives us confidence that any sample can represent the whole population (see our Methods 101 explainer on random sampling). Here are the questions used for the survey of Black adults, along with its responses and methodology .

The terms “Black Americans,” “Black people” and “Black adults” are used interchangeably throughout this report to refer to U.S. adults who self-identify as Black, either alone or in combination with other races or Hispanic identity.

Throughout this report, “Black, non-Hispanic” respondents are those who identify as single-race Black and say they have no Hispanic background. “Black Hispanic” respondents are those who identify as Black and say they have Hispanic background. We use the terms “Black Hispanic” and “Hispanic Black” interchangeably. “Multiracial” respondents are those who indicate two or more racial backgrounds (one of which is Black) and say they are not Hispanic.

Respondents were asked a question about how important being Black was to how they think about themselves. In this report, we use the term “being Black” when referencing responses to this question.

In this report, “immigrant” refers to people who were not U.S. citizens at birth – in other words, those born outside the U.S., Puerto Rico or other U.S. territories to parents who were not U.S. citizens. We use the terms “immigrant,” “born abroad” and “foreign-born” interchangeably.

Throughout this report, “Democrats and Democratic leaners” and just “Democrats” both refer to respondents who identify politically with the Democratic Party or who are independent or some other party but lean toward the Democratic Party. “Republicans and Republican leaners” and just “Republicans” both refer to respondents who identify politically with the Republican Party or are independent or some other party but lean toward the Republican Party.

Respondents were asked a question about their voter registration status. In this report, respondents are considered registered to vote if they self-report being absolutely certain they are registered at their current address. Respondents are considered not registered to vote if they report not being registered or express uncertainty about their registration.

To create the upper-, middle- and lower-income tiers, respondents’ 2020 family incomes were adjusted for differences in purchasing power by geographic region and household size. Respondents were then placed into income tiers: “Middle income” is defined as two-thirds to double the median annual income for the entire survey sample. “Lower income” falls below that range, and “upper income” lies above it. For more information about how the income tiers were created, read the methodology .

Bar chart showing after George Floyd’s murder, half of Black Americans expected policy changes to address racial inequality, After George Floyd’s murder, half of Black Americans expected policy changes to address racial inequality

More than a year after the murder of George Floyd and the national protests, debate and political promises that ensued, 65% of Black Americans say the increased national attention on racial inequality has not led to changes that improved their lives. 1 And 44% say equality for Black people in the United States is not likely to be achieved, according to newly released findings from an October 2021 survey of Black Americans by Pew Research Center.

This is somewhat of a reversal in views from September 2020, when half of Black adults said the increased national focus on issues of race would lead to major policy changes to address racial inequality in the country and 56% expected changes that would make their lives better.

At the same time, many Black Americans are concerned about racial discrimination and its impact. Roughly eight-in-ten say they have personally experienced discrimination because of their race or ethnicity (79%), and most also say discrimination is the main reason many Black people cannot get ahead (68%).  

Even so, Black Americans have a clear vision for how to achieve change when it comes to racial inequality. This includes support for significant reforms to or complete overhauls of several U.S. institutions to ensure fair treatment, particularly the criminal justice system; political engagement, primarily in the form of voting; support for Black businesses to advance Black communities; and reparations in the forms of educational, business and homeownership assistance. Yet alongside their assessments of inequality and ideas about progress exists pessimism about whether U.S. society and its institutions will change in ways that would reduce racism.

These findings emerge from an extensive Pew Research Center survey of 3,912 Black Americans conducted online Oct. 4-17, 2021. The survey explores how Black Americans assess their position in U.S. society and their ideas about social change. Overall, Black Americans are clear on what they think the problems are facing the country and how to remedy them. However, they are skeptical that meaningful changes will take place in their lifetime.

Black Americans see racism in our laws as a big problem and discrimination as a roadblock to progress

Bar chart showing about six-in-ten Black adults say racism and police brutality are extremely big problems for Black people in the U.S. today

Black adults were asked in the survey to assess the current nature of racism in the United States and whether structural or individual sources of this racism are a bigger problem for Black people. About half of Black adults (52%) say racism in our laws is a bigger problem than racism by individual people, while four-in-ten (43%) say acts of racism committed by individual people is the bigger problem. Only 3% of Black adults say that Black people do not experience discrimination in the U.S. today.

In assessing the magnitude of problems that they face, the majority of Black Americans say racism (63%), police brutality (60%) and economic inequality (54%) are extremely or very big problems for Black people living in the U.S. Slightly smaller shares say the same about the affordability of health care (47%), limitations on voting (46%), and the quality of K-12 schools (40%).

Aside from their critiques of U.S. institutions, Black adults also feel the impact of racial inequality personally. Most Black adults say they occasionally or frequently experience unfair treatment because of their race or ethnicity (79%), and two-thirds (68%) cite racial discrimination as the main reason many Black people cannot get ahead today.

Black Americans’ views on reducing racial inequality

Bar chart showing many Black adults say institutional overhauls are necessary to ensure fair treatment

Black Americans are clear on the challenges they face because of racism. They are also clear on the solutions. These range from overhauls of policing practices and the criminal justice system to civic engagement and reparations to descendants of people enslaved in the United States.

Changing U.S. institutions such as policing, courts and prison systems

About nine-in-ten Black adults say multiple aspects of the criminal justice system need some kind of change (minor, major or a complete overhaul) to ensure fair treatment, with nearly all saying so about policing (95%), the courts and judicial process (95%), and the prison system (94%).

Roughly half of Black adults say policing (49%), the courts and judicial process (48%), and the prison system (54%) need to be completely rebuilt for Black people to be treated fairly. Smaller shares say the same about the political system (42%), the economic system (37%) and the health care system (34%), according to the October survey.

While Black Americans are in favor of significant changes to policing, most want spending on police departments in their communities to stay the same (39%) or increase (35%). A little more than one-in-five (23%) think spending on police departments in their area should be decreased.

Black adults who favor decreases in police spending are most likely to name medical, mental health and social services (40%) as the top priority for those reappropriated funds. Smaller shares say K-12 schools (25%), roads, water systems and other infrastructure (12%), and reducing taxes (13%) should be the top priority.

Voting and ‘buying Black’ viewed as important strategies for Black community advancement

Black Americans also have clear views on the types of political and civic engagement they believe will move Black communities forward. About six-in-ten Black adults say voting (63%) and supporting Black businesses or “buying Black” (58%) are extremely or very effective strategies for moving Black people toward equality in the U.S. Smaller though still significant shares say the same about volunteering with organizations dedicated to Black equality (48%), protesting (42%) and contacting elected officials (40%).

Black adults were also asked about the effectiveness of Black economic and political independence in moving them toward equality. About four-in-ten (39%) say Black ownership of all businesses in Black neighborhoods would be an extremely or very effective strategy for moving toward racial equality, while roughly three-in-ten (31%) say the same about establishing a national Black political party. And about a quarter of Black adults (27%) say having Black neighborhoods governed entirely by Black elected officials would be extremely or very effective in moving Black people toward equality.

Most Black Americans support repayment for slavery

Discussions about atonement for slavery predate the founding of the United States. As early as 1672 , Quaker abolitionists advocated for enslaved people to be paid for their labor once they were free. And in recent years, some U.S. cities and institutions have implemented reparations policies to do just that.

Most Black Americans say the legacy of slavery affects the position of Black people in the U.S. either a great deal (55%) or a fair amount (30%), according to the survey. And roughly three-quarters (77%) say descendants of people enslaved in the U.S. should be repaid in some way.

Black adults who say descendants of the enslaved should be repaid support doing so in different ways. About eight-in-ten say repayment in the forms of educational scholarships (80%), financial assistance for starting or improving a business (77%), and financial assistance for buying or remodeling a home (76%) would be extremely or very helpful. A slightly smaller share (69%) say cash payments would be extremely or very helpful forms of repayment for the descendants of enslaved people.

Where the responsibility for repayment lies is also clear for Black Americans. Among those who say the descendants of enslaved people should be repaid, 81% say the U.S. federal government should have all or most of the responsibility for repayment. About three-quarters (76%) say businesses and banks that profited from slavery should bear all or most of the responsibility for repayment. And roughly six-in-ten say the same about colleges and universities that benefited from slavery (63%) and descendants of families who engaged in the slave trade (60%).

Black Americans are skeptical change will happen

Bar chart showing little hope among Black adults that changes to address racial inequality are likely

Even though Black Americans’ visions for social change are clear, very few expect them to be implemented. Overall, 44% of Black adults say equality for Black people in the U.S. is a little or not at all likely. A little over a third (38%) say it is somewhat likely and only 13% say it is extremely or very likely.

They also do not think specific institutions will change. Two-thirds of Black adults say changes to the prison system (67%) and the courts and judicial process (65%) that would ensure fair treatment for Black people are a little or not at all likely in their lifetime. About six-in-ten (58%) say the same about policing. Only about one-in-ten say changes to policing (13%), the courts and judicial process (12%), and the prison system (11%) are extremely or very likely.

This pessimism is not only about the criminal justice system. The majority of Black adults say the political (63%), economic (62%) and health care (51%) systems are also unlikely to change in their lifetime.

Black Americans’ vision for social change includes reparations. However, much like their pessimism about institutional change, very few think they will see reparations in their lifetime. Among Black adults who say the descendants of people enslaved in the U.S. should be repaid, 82% say reparations for slavery are unlikely to occur in their lifetime. About one-in-ten (11%) say repayment is somewhat likely, while only 7% say repayment is extremely or very likely to happen in their lifetime.

Black Democrats, Republicans differ on assessments of inequality and visions for social change

Bar chart showing Black adults differ by party in their views on racial discrimination and changes to policing

Party affiliation is one key point of difference among Black Americans in their assessments of racial inequality and their visions for social change. Black Republicans and Republican leaners are more likely than Black Democrats and Democratic leaners to focus on the acts of individuals. For example, when summarizing the nature of racism against Black people in the U.S., the majority of Black Republicans (59%) say racist acts committed by individual people is a bigger problem for Black people than racism in our laws. Black Democrats (41%) are less likely to hold this view.

Black Republicans (45%) are also more likely than Black Democrats (21%) to say that Black people who cannot get ahead in the U.S. are mostly responsible for their own condition. And while similar shares of Black Republicans (79%) and Democrats (80%) say they experience racial discrimination on a regular basis, Republicans (64%) are more likely than Democrats (36%) to say that most Black people who want to get ahead can make it if they are willing to work hard.

On the other hand, Black Democrats are more likely than Black Republicans to focus on the impact that racial inequality has on Black Americans. Seven-in-ten Black Democrats (73%) say racial discrimination is the main reason many Black people cannot get ahead in the U.S, while about four-in-ten Black Republicans (44%) say the same. And Black Democrats are more likely than Black Republicans to say racism (67% vs. 46%) and police brutality (65% vs. 44%) are extremely big problems for Black people today.

Black Democrats are also more critical of U.S. institutions than Black Republicans are. For example, Black Democrats are more likely than Black Republicans to say the prison system (57% vs. 35%), policing (52% vs. 29%) and the courts and judicial process (50% vs. 35%) should be completely rebuilt for Black people to be treated fairly.

While the share of Black Democrats who want to see large-scale changes to the criminal justice system exceeds that of Black Republicans, they share similar views on police funding. Four-in-ten each of Black Democrats and Black Republicans say funding for police departments in their communities should remain the same, while around a third of each partisan coalition (36% and 37%, respectively) says funding should increase. Only about one-in-four Black Democrats (24%) and one-in-five Black Republicans (21%) say funding for police departments in their communities should decrease.

Among the survey’s other findings:

Black adults differ by age in their views on political strategies. Black adults ages 65 and older (77%) are most likely to say voting is an extremely or very effective strategy for moving Black people toward equality. They are significantly more likely than Black adults ages 18 to 29 (48%) and 30 to 49 (60%) to say this. Black adults 65 and older (48%) are also more likely than those ages 30 to 49 (38%) and 50 to 64 (42%) to say protesting is an extremely or very effective strategy. Roughly four-in-ten Black adults ages 18 to 29 say this (44%).

Gender plays a role in how Black adults view policing. Though majorities of Black women (65%) and men (56%) say police brutality is an extremely big problem for Black people living in the U.S. today, Black women are more likely than Black men to hold this view. When it comes to criminal justice, Black women (56%) and men (51%) are about equally likely to share the view that the prison system should be completely rebuilt to ensure fair treatment of Black people. However, Black women (52%) are slightly more likely than Black men (45%) to say this about policing. On the matter of police funding, Black women (39%) are slightly more likely than Black men (31%) to say police funding in their communities should be increased. On the other hand, Black men are more likely than Black women to prefer that funding stay the same (44% vs. 36%). Smaller shares of both Black men (23%) and women (22%) would like to see police funding decreased.

Income impacts Black adults’ views on reparations. Roughly eight-in-ten Black adults with lower (78%), middle (77%) and upper incomes (79%) say the descendants of people enslaved in the U.S. should receive reparations. Among those who support reparations, Black adults with upper and middle incomes (both 84%) are more likely than those with lower incomes (75%) to say educational scholarships would be an extremely or very helpful form of repayment. However, of those who support reparations, Black adults with lower (72%) and middle incomes (68%) are more likely than those with higher incomes (57%) to say cash payments would be an extremely or very helpful form of repayment for slavery.

  • Black adults in the September 2020 survey only include those who say their race is Black alone and are non-Hispanic. The same is true only for the questions of improvements to Black people’s lives and equality in the United States in the October 2021 survey. Throughout the rest of this report, Black adults include those who say their race is Black alone and non-Hispanic; those who say their race is Black and at least one other race and non-Hispanic; or Black and Hispanic, unless otherwise noted. ↩

Sign up for our weekly newsletter

Fresh data delivery Saturday mornings

Sign up for The Briefing

Weekly updates on the world of news & information

  • Black Americans
  • Discrimination & Prejudice
  • Economic Inequality
  • Race, Ethnicity & Politics

Black voters support Harris over Trump and Kennedy by a wide margin

Most black americans believe u.s. institutions were designed to hold black people back, among black adults, those with higher incomes are most likely to say they are happy, black americans’ experiences with news, 8 facts about black lives matter, most popular, report materials.

  • American Trends Panel Wave 97

901 E St. NW, Suite 300 Washington, DC 20004 USA (+1) 202-419-4300 | Main (+1) 202-857-8562 | Fax (+1) 202-419-4372 |  Media Inquiries

Research Topics

  • Email Newsletters

ABOUT PEW RESEARCH CENTER  Pew Research Center is a nonpartisan fact tank that informs the public about the issues, attitudes and trends shaping the world. It conducts public opinion polling, demographic research, media content analysis and other empirical social science research. Pew Research Center does not take policy positions. It is a subsidiary of  The Pew Charitable Trusts .

© 2024 Pew Research Center

Encyclopedia Britannica

  • History & Society
  • Science & Tech
  • Biographies
  • Animals & Nature
  • Geography & Travel
  • Arts & Culture
  • Games & Quizzes
  • On This Day
  • One Good Fact
  • New Articles
  • Lifestyles & Social Issues
  • Philosophy & Religion
  • Politics, Law & Government
  • World History
  • Health & Medicine
  • Browse Biographies
  • Birds, Reptiles & Other Vertebrates
  • Bugs, Mollusks & Other Invertebrates
  • Environment
  • Fossils & Geologic Time
  • Entertainment & Pop Culture
  • Sports & Recreation
  • Visual Arts
  • Demystified
  • Image Galleries
  • Infographics
  • Top Questions
  • Britannica Kids
  • Saving Earth
  • Space Next 50
  • Student Center

racially restricted beach in apartheid-era South Africa

What is racism?

What are some of the societal aspects of racism, what are some of the measures taken to combat racism.

Sheet music cover 'Jim Crow Jubilee' illustrated with caricatures of African-American musicians and dancers. Originally, Jim Crow was a character in a song by Thomas Rice. (racism, segregation)

Our editors will review what you’ve submitted and determine whether to revise the article.

  • Social Sciences LibreTexts - Racism
  • Academia - Racial Discrimination and Redlining in Cities
  • GlobalSecurity.org - Racism
  • PBS LearningMedia - American Experience - A Class Apart: The Birth and Growth of Racism Against Mexican-Americans
  • Frontiers - Racism and censorship in the editorial and peer review process
  • United Nations - The Ideology of Racism: Misusing science to justify racial discrimination
  • National Endowment for the Humanities - Humanities - El Movimiento
  • PBS - Frontline - A Class Divided - Documentary Introduction
  • Cornell Law School - Legal Information Institute - Racism
  • racism - Children's Encyclopedia (Ages 8-11)
  • racism - Student Encyclopedia (Ages 11 and up)

racially restricted beach in apartheid-era South Africa

Racism is the belief that humans can be divided into separate and exclusive biological entities called “races”; that there is a causal link between inherited physical traits and traits of personality, intellect, morality, and other cultural and behavioral features; and that some races are innately superior to others. Racism was at the heart of North American slavery and the colonization and empire-building activities of western Europeans, especially in the 18th century. Since the late 20th century the notion of biological race has been recognized as a cultural invention, entirely without scientific basis. Most human societies have concluded that racism is wrong, and social trends have moved away from racism.

Historically, the practice of racism held that members of low-status “races” should be limited to low-status jobs or enslavement and be excluded from access to political power, economic resources, and unrestricted civil rights. The lived experience of racism for members of low-status races includes acts of physical violence, daily insults, and frequent acts and verbal expressions of contempt and disrespect.

Racism elicits hatred and distrust and precludes any attempt to understand its victims. Many societies attempt to combat racism by raising awareness of racist beliefs and practices and by promoting human understanding in public policies. The Universal Declaration of Human Rights , adopted by the United Nations in 1948, is an example of one measure taken to combat racism. In the United States, the civil rights movement ’s fight against racism gained national prominence during the 1950s and has had lasting positive effects.

Recent News

racism , the belief that humans may be divided into separate and exclusive biological entities called “races”; that there is a causal link between inherited physical traits and traits of personality, intellect, morality , and other cultural and behavioral features; and that some races are innately superior to others. The term is also applied to political, economic, or legal institutions and systems that engage in or perpetuate discrimination on the basis of race or otherwise reinforce racial inequalities in wealth and income, education , health care, civil rights, and other areas. Such institutional, structural, or systemic racism became a particular focus of scholarly investigation in the 1980s with the emergence of critical race theory , an offshoot of the critical legal studies movement. Since the late 20th century the notion of biological race has been recognized as a cultural invention, entirely without scientific basis.

Following Germany’s defeat in World War I , that country’s deeply ingrained anti-Semitism was successfully exploited by the Nazi Party , which seized power in 1933 and implemented policies of systematic discrimination, persecution, and eventual mass murder of Jews in Germany and in the territories occupied by the country during World War II ( see Holocaust ).

Martin Luther King, Jr. (center), with other civil rights supporters lock arms on as they lead the way along Constitution Avenue during the March on Washington, Washington, D.C., on August 28, 1963.

In North America and apartheid -era South Africa , racism dictated that different races (chiefly blacks and whites) should be segregated from one another; that they should have their own distinct communities and develop their own institutions such as churches, schools, and hospitals; and that it was unnatural for members of different races to marry .

Historically, those who openly professed or practiced racism held that members of low-status races should be limited to low-status jobs and that members of the dominant race should have exclusive access to political power, economic resources, high-status jobs, and unrestricted civil rights . The lived experience of racism for members of low-status races includes acts of physical violence , daily insults, and frequent acts and verbal expressions of contempt and disrespect, all of which have profound effects on self-esteem and social relationships.

Racism was at the heart of North American slavery and the colonization and empire-building activities of western Europeans, especially in the 18th century. The idea of race was invented to magnify the differences between people of European origin and those of African descent whose ancestors had been involuntarily enslaved and transported to the Americas. By characterizing Africans and their African American descendants as lesser human beings, the proponents of slavery attempted to justify and maintain the system of exploitation while portraying the United States as a bastion and champion of human freedom, with human rights , democratic institutions, unlimited opportunities, and equality. The contradiction between slavery and the ideology of human equality, accompanying a philosophy of human freedom and dignity, seemed to demand the dehumanization of those enslaved.

essay on racial bias

By the 19th century, racism had matured and spread around the world. In many countries, leaders began to think of the ethnic components of their own societies, usually religious or language groups, in racial terms and to designate “higher” and “lower” races. Those seen as the low-status races, especially in colonized areas, were exploited for their labour, and discrimination against them became a common pattern in many areas of the world. The expressions and feelings of racial superiority that accompanied colonialism generated resentment and hostility from those who were colonized and exploited, feelings that continued even after independence.

essay on racial bias

Since the mid-20th century many conflicts around the world have been interpreted in racial terms even though their origins were in the ethnic hostilities that have long characterized many human societies (e.g., Arabs and Jews, English and Irish). Racism reflects an acceptance of the deepest forms and degrees of divisiveness and carries the implication that differences between groups are so great that they cannot be transcended .

Racism elicits hatred and distrust and precludes any attempt to understand its victims. For that reason, most human societies have concluded that racism is wrong, at least in principle, and social trends have moved away from racism. Many societies have begun to combat racism by raising awareness of racist beliefs and practices and by promoting human understanding in public policies, as does the Universal Declaration of Human Rights , set forth by the United Nations in 1948.

essay on racial bias

In the United States, racism came under increasing attack during the civil rights movement of the 1950s and ’60s, and laws and social policies that enforced racial segregation and permitted racial discrimination against African Americans were gradually eliminated. Laws aimed at limiting the voting power of racial minorities were invalidated by the Twenty-fourth Amendment (1964) to the U.S. Constitution , which prohibited poll taxes , and by the federal Voting Rights Act (1965), which required jurisdictions with a history of voter suppression to obtain federal approval (“preclearance”) of any proposed changes to their voting laws (the preclearance requirement was effectively removed by the U.S. Supreme Court in 2013 [ see Shelby County v. Holder ]). By 2020 nearly three-quarters of the states had adopted varying forms of voter ID law , by which would-be voters were required or requested to present certain forms of identification before casting a ballot. Critics of the laws, some of which were successfully challenged in the courts, contended that they effectively suppressed voting among African Americans and other demographic groups. Other measures that tended to limit voting by African Americans were unconstitutional racial gerrymanders , partisan gerrymanders aimed at limiting the number of Democratic representatives in state legislatures and Congress, the closing of polling stations in African American or Democratic-leaning neighbourhoods, restrictions on the use of mail-in and absentee ballots, limits on early voting, and purges of voter rolls.

Despite constitutional and legal measures aimed at protecting the rights of racial minorities in the United States, the private beliefs and practices of many Americans remained racist, and some group of assumed lower status was often made a scapegoat. That tendency has persisted well into the 21st century.

Because, in the popular mind, “race” is linked to physical differences among peoples, and such features as dark skin colour have been seen as markers of low status, some experts believe that racism may be difficult to eradicate . Indeed, minds cannot be changed by laws, but beliefs about human differences can and do change, as do all cultural elements.

  • IPR Intranet

INSTITUTE FOR POLICY RESEARCH

  • What We Study
  • Race, Poverty, and Inequality
  • Race, Interracial Relations & Prejudice

Race, Racial Bias, and Prejudice

Public perceptions of black girls and their punitive consequences .

Black girl sitting at a desk with a phone

Knowledge About People’s Interracial Friendships Influences How They Are Viewed 

People’s biases about other groups are shaped by their interactions with and by characteristics of those groups. In  Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , Kellogg social psychologist and IPR associate  Ivuoma Onyeador  and her colleagues conduct four studies to  investigate  how interracial friendships affect how an individual is viewed. The first two studies sought to determine whether perceptions of an individual’s race changed based on their assumed friendship circles. The approximately 180 participants, split between Black and White Americans, were shown two images at a time of either Black or White Americans’ faces with random noise added and were asked to select the image most likely to be a person with mostly Black, mostly White, or an equal number of Black and White friends. Those selections were then averaged to create a “mental representation” of Black and White people whose friendship network varied in its racial makeup. Participants perceived Black individuals with mostly Black friends to have darker skin than Black individuals with mostly White friends and perceived White individuals with more Black friends to have darker skin than White individuals with mostly White friends, which the researchers termed the racial assimilation effect. In the third study, 120 White participants rated how African or European individuals appeared, perceiving the images determined to have mostly Black friends as more African and those determined to have mostly White friends as more European. In the fourth, 102 participants, half Black and half White, rated how African and European the images looked, and how threatening, trustworthy, warm, competent, or lower class the individuals appeared. Study four found that in general, participants rated images of an individual generated by participants who believed the person had mostly Black friends more negatively than an individual with mixed or mostly White friends. The one exception was evaluations of White individuals with mostly White friends generated by Black participants. The research demonstrates that interracial friendships influence perceptions of race, group loyalty, and traits. Additionally, because Black and White participants positively viewed those whom they perceived to have other-race friends, the researchers suggest interracial friendships may influence perceptions of interracial solidarity.

The Role of Parents in Developing the Racial Identities of Multiracial Black Young Adults

Racial identity is a crucial part of identity development that is shaped by different environments, including the home. In  Race and Social Problems , IPR graduate student Courtney Meiling Jones and IPR developmental psychologist  Onnie Rogers   explored  the role parents play in Multiracial, Black young adults making meaning of their own racial identities. The researchers examined 11 semi-structured interviews of Multiracial Black college students about race, racial identity, and the Black Lives Matter movement taken from the “Beyond Black and White” study. The researchers used the m(ai)cro model to focus on the role of the macrosystem in human development, allowing them to analyze the ways in which societal structures of power cannot be separated from the micro-level racial identities and socialization experiences of Multiracial youth. Similarly, the researchers relied on Critical Multiracial Theory (MultiCrit) to assess the distinct ways that the structure of white supremacy influences multiraciality and how this influence forms the everyday experiences and individual assessment of Multiracial people. The researchers find college students spontaneously mentioned their parents in the interviews as a source of racial information or guidance, as a way to support their own racial identification, and to illustrate how their Multiracial identities created moments of connection and disconnection with parents and family members. The findings underscore that parents play a pivotal role in helping Multiracial youth navigate their identities into the young adult years. Moreover, the young adults show us how parents of Multiracial young people can best support their children’s racial development, including by being aware of how their own racial identities as parents may support or constrain how their children are able to identify in the American racial context. Future research should continue to explore the role of families in socializing their children—especially Multiracial youth.

Studying Racial/ Ethnic Identity Among White Youth

White youth, like all youth in the United States, grow up in a system of racial inequity, yet little research exists about how they make sense of their racial identity. IPR developmental psychologist  Onnie Rogers  and Ursula Moffitt of Wheaton College Massachusetts  offer  insights on framing and conducting research on how White youth understand their own racial identity in the  Journal of Research on Adolescence . The researchers discuss the history of research on racial and ethnic identity development and argue that it is necessary to use different measures to understand racial identity development among White youth versus Black or Latinx youth. Helms’ White Racial Identity Development (WRID) model, or a two-phase model that examines internalization and challenges to the racist status quo, offers one framework for a contextualized study of White identity. The researchers used this model in previous research and suggest that children are aware of race at a young age, but their ability to make sense of race and racism can change with experiential knowledge and greater cognitive ability. Finally, the researchers urge scholars to use models designed to assess Whiteness in context, move beyond age-related changes to explain development, reflect on how their own identities shape their work, and apply an anti-racist lens when using terms and constructs when conducting research on racial identity among White youth.

Directions for Vaccinating Children Against Racism: Treating Racism as a Virus

During the COVID-19 pandemic, which disproportionately affected Black Americans, many around the United States watched the murder of George Floyd by a police officer, a racist experience that was not new for Black youth. In the  Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology , pediatrician and IPR associate  Nia Heard-Garris  and her colleagues  discuss  how racism is a social virus analogous to infectious viruses such as the coronavirus that causes COVID-19. Structural racism is embedded within our societal structures and unfairly advantages certain racial or ethnic groups over others, and this is a common lived experience of racially minoritized groups in the U.S. The researchers note that viruses, which need a host to survive, impact more susceptible people, and in the same way, people who seek dominance and rely on patterned behaviors are likely to be “host” for racist behaviors. The authors argue that racism is comparable to an endemic virus that continues to circulate because it is woven into the fabric of American society, including the medical profession. Viruses can be pandemic or endemic, and the authors point out that racism is endemic in American society. Racism can spread through harmful practices, like when parents reinforce racial hierarchies or promote mistrust of racial groups. These harmful practices can negatively impact both the physical and mental health of children. The authors argue that parents should socialize their children about race in healthy ways to acknowledge racism within society and reduce the spread of prejudice. They also suggest mental health professionals receive training programs to combat racism, which can support the wellbeing of children and adolescents.

Researching the Psychology of Racial Bias Using a Bidirectional Approach

The field of psychology often focuses on the study of racial bias through the lens of the individual, yet this approach largely ignores systemic racism and the way bias is shaped by broader cultural systems. In  Nature Reviews Psychology , IPR psychologist  Sylvia Perry  and her colleagues  examine  the bidirectional relationship between individual-level biases and systems and structures that create and reinforce racial inequality. The researchers examine five systemic factors that relate to the individual level: power and privilege disparities, cultural narratives and values, segregated communities, shared stereotypes, and nonverbal messages. For example, when it comes to systems around cultural narratives and values, American history is often taught through a White-affirming lens in schools, obscuring the relationship between contemporary systemic injustice and racial injustices of the past. This can shape individual attitudes about race and racism. Individual-level bias can also impact how history and culture are presented, based on how an educator selects and constructs narratives. This example highlights the way systemic and individual level biases influence and play into one another. The researchers propose several intervention recommendations to reduce racial bias, including for White parents to talk to their children about race and racism and better education about the history and systems that have led to current racial injustices and inequalities. Future psychology research should test interventions to reduce racial bias that targets both systems and individuals.

Building Support for Parent-Child Conversations About Race

White mom with daughts

Racial Socialization Messages in White Parents’ Conversations About Current Events with Their Children

Given the rise of white nationalism since the 2016 presidential election, researchers have identified an urgent need to understand the conversations White families have—or do not have—about race and racism. In the Journal of Research on Adolescence,  IPR psychologist  Sylvia Perry  and researchers from the University of Vermont examine  how White parents communicate messages about race and racism to their teenage children when discussing current events involving racism. In the first study, conducted in September 2019, the researchers asked 123 White parents to answer questions about their attitudes toward and interactions with various social groups, and how they discuss social groups and current events involving police brutality and white supremacy with their child aged 14–17. In the second study, conducted in June 2020, the researchers asked 104 White parents of teenagers ages 14–17 if and how they discussed the deaths of George Floyd, Breonna Taylor, Ahmad Arbery, and other Black Americans killed by police with their child. The researchers find that the rates of discussions in 2020 (79–81%) were double those in 2019 (40–43%). Despite this finding, color-conscious messages, which show awareness of racism, were less common in 2020 than in 2019, and color-blind messages, which deny race and racism are real or important, were similarly prevalent across both groups. The evidence shows that while the higher rates of conversations in 2020 suggests progress toward racial justice, the content of the conversations suggest otherwise. The researchers write that the next step is to identify intervention points that will help redirect White parents toward more color-conscious conversations.

IMAGES

  1. Racial Discrimination Research Paper Free Essay Example

    essay on racial bias

  2. ≫ Racial Prejudice and Discrimination Free Essay Sample on Samploon.com

    essay on racial bias

  3. Argumentative Essay On Racial Profiling

    essay on racial bias

  4. Racial Bias Argumentative Essay Sample

    essay on racial bias

  5. The Racial Bias in the 21st Century

    essay on racial bias

  6. Racial Discrimination In Modern World

    essay on racial bias

VIDEO

  1. Bias & Racism

  2. Know Your Bias: Groupthink and the Algorithm Influence

  3. Racial conflict poser from school textbook, says MP

  4. Racial Discrimination Essay//BSEB Class 12th English Essay for Exam//Nishant Sir

COMMENTS

  1. The Impact of Racism on the Society: [Essay Example], 2796 words

    Studies show that experiencing racial bias has had profound effects on people's health and welfare. The effects can include feelings of sadness and anger, even anxiety and depression. The regular experience of racism can lead to people withdrawing from work or study, and diminish their quality of life.

  2. 398 Racism Essay Titles & Writing Examples

    Anti-Racism: Marginalization and Exclusion in Healthcare. This essay examines the course's impact and the concepts of marginalization and exclusion in healthcare. Marginalization is a concept that has profoundly influenced the understanding of race and racism in healthcare. The Issue of Racism in the United States.

  3. Working together against racism

    Much of the public used to think that only discriminatory laws or overt acts of interpersonal discrimination, such as the use of racial slurs, counted as racism. But today, many people recognize that systemic disadvantage and more subtle microaggressions are also a key part of the racial-minority experience in America and cause great harm.

  4. 150 Essay Topics On Racism to Help You Compose an Essay

    3678 (20 min read) Here's a list of 150 essay ideas on racism to help you ace a perfect paper. The subjects are divided based on what you require! Before we continue with the list of essay topics on racism, let's remember the definition of racism. In brief, it's a complex prejudice and a form of discrimination based on race.

  5. Racism, bias, and discrimination

    Racism, bias, and discrimination. Racism is a form of prejudice that generally includes negative emotional reactions to members of a group, acceptance of negative stereotypes, and racial discrimination against individuals; in some cases it can lead to violence. Discrimination refers to the differential treatment of different age, gender, racial ...

  6. Taking a hard look at our implicit biases

    Banaji opened on Tuesday by recounting the "implicit association" experiments she had done at Yale and at Harvard. The assumptions underlying the research on implicit bias derive from well-established theories of learning and memory and the empirical results are derived from tasks that have their roots in experimental psychology and ...

  7. Racism in the Structure of Everyday Worlds: A Cultural-Psychological

    The term racism is often used synonymously with prejudice (biased feelings or affect), stereotyping (biased thoughts and beliefs, flawed generalizations), discrimination (differential treatment or the absence of equal treatment), and bigotry (intolerance or hatred). This practice implicitly conceptualizes racism as a set of basic social-psychological processes underlying the psychologies of ...

  8. PDF Racism, Sociology of

    Abstract. The sociology of racism is the study of the relationship between racism, racial discrimination, and racial inequality. While past scholarship emphasized overtly racist attitudes and policies, contemporary sociology considers racism as individual- and group-level processes and structures that are implicated in the reproduction of ...

  9. The Far-Reaching Impacts of Racism and Bias

    In 2010 a group of researchers conducted an experiment in which they pretended to be students, emailing 6,500 professors in nearly 90 academic fields at 259 universities with a request to discuss research before applying for a doctoral program. The text of the messages was identical, but the sender's name was varied to signal race and gender, such as Brad Anderson, Latoya Brown, and Mei Chen.

  10. A Systematic Review of Black People Coping With Racism: Approaches

    Racial discrimination occurs when a person is mistreated because of their perceived race or ethnic group (Haeny et al., 2021).A person who is regularly exposed to racial discrimination must integrate coping mechanisms into their everyday life to combat the many and ongoing adverse effects associated with race-based stress and trauma.

  11. Essays on Racial Discrimination

    The United States of America is thought to be the "land of the free.". Freedom is what one thinks of the U.S., but there is a long history of millions of African Americans that were treated unfairly and worse than people of the Caucasian race... Oppression in America Oppression Racial Discrimination. 16.

  12. Examining systemic racism, advancing racial equity

    As questions about racial bias in the criminal justice system dominate the headlines, research by Stanford law Professor John J. Donohue III offers insight into one of the most fraught areas: the ...

  13. A systemic approach to the psychology of racial bias within ...

    The field of psychology so far has primarily focused on racial bias at an individual level, centring the effects of various stimuli on the racial biases of individuals 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11 ...

  14. Cause and Effect of Racial Discrimination Essay

    A scientific study by Carter et al. demonstrates that this discrimination type significantly harms people's psychological well-being. For example, victims of this phenomenon report increased rates of depression, stress, anger, anxiety, psychological distress, low self-esteem, substance abuse, somatization, and many others (Carter et al. 26).

  15. Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education Essay

    Get a custom essay on Racial Discrimination and Justice in Education. A significant factor in systemic racism in modern schools is the theory of colorblindness as the prevailing ideology in schools and pedagogical universities. The total avoidance of racial topics in schools has led to a complete absence of material related to the culture of ...

  16. Personal Essays About Casual Racism With Friends And Family ...

    We delve into the issue on this week's episode of the Code Switch podcast, featuring writer Nicole Chung and Code Switch's Shereen Marisol Meraji, Gene Demby and Karen Grigsby Bates. We also asked ...

  17. How Structural Racism Works

    Racial bias in pain assessment and treatment recommendations, and false beliefs about biological differences between blacks and whites. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016;113:4296-4301. Crossref

  18. Full article: Resisting racism in everyday life: from ignoring to

    Racism and racial discrimination. The idea of races maintained through notions of different phenotypes, ethnicities and cultures, produces a variety of forms of racism (Lentin Citation 2008).Broadly speaking, racism involves assigning people negative characteristics based on their "real or imaginary" difference, thus depicting them as subordinate and using that subordination to legitimate ...

  19. Systemic racism: individuals and interactions, institutions and society

    Moreover, studying racial bias is interesting; it will improve the science; and it is the obvious path to ensuring a mutually respectful, peaceful society that flourishes economically, politically, and socially. ... A useful metaphor guiding the essay. There are these two fish swimming along and they happen to meet an older fish swimming the ...

  20. Albert Einstein's Essay on Racial Bias in 1946

    Albert Einstein's Essay on Racial Bias in 1946. Trent T. Gilliss. July 12, 2016. In the years after World War II, Albert Einstein took up the mantle of confronting racism in America. He became a good friend and comrade of the prominent opera singer Paul Robeson, co-chaired an anti-lynching campaign, and was an outspoken supporter of W.E.B. Du ...

  21. Black Americans' Views of Racial Inequality, Racism, Reparations and

    Seven-in-ten Black Democrats (73%) say racial discrimination is the main reason many Black people cannot get ahead in the U.S, while about four-in-ten Black Republicans (44%) say the same. And Black Democrats are more likely than Black Republicans to say racism (67% vs. 46%) and police brutality (65% vs. 44%) are extremely big problems for ...

  22. Racism

    racism, the belief that humans may be divided into separate and exclusive biological entities called "races"; that there is a causal link between inherited physical traits and traits of personality, intellect, morality, and other cultural and behavioral features; and that some races are innately superior to others.

  23. Race, Racial Bias, and Prejudice: Institute for Policy Research

    The researchers examined 11 semi-structured interviews of Multiracial Black college students about race, racial identity, and the Black Lives Matter movement taken from the "Beyond Black and White" study. The researchers used the m (ai)cro model to focus on the role of the macrosystem in human development, allowing them to analyze the ways ...

  24. Revealing racial bias

    Essay. Social Sciences. Share on. Revealing racial bias. Dean Knox [email protected] Authors Info & Affiliations. Science. 4 Nov 2021. Vol 374, Issue 6568. ... Courts and city councils struggle to measure the severity of racial bias in policing, let alone to identify the means to address such bias. Solutions are difficult to identify because ...